2016
DOI: 10.1167/16.15.23
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Accuracy of eye position for saccades and smooth pursuit

Abstract: In this study, we address the question of whether a target is foveated during smooth pursuit. Specifically, we examine whether smooth pursuit eye movements land near the center-of-mass of the target, as is the case for saccades. To that end, we instructed eight untrained, healthy participants to follow moving targets, presented monocularly in a scanning laser ophthalmoscope. Stimuli moved either in a modified step-ramp (smooth pursuit), or made a single step (saccade), stepping 6° from the center. Targets were… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

1
14
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
1
14
0
Order By: Relevance
“…A lower accuracy would suggest that the target was not being continuously foveated. Indeed, this would be consistent with the results of Shanidze et al,14 who found that small targets are not always foveated during pursuit. In typical observers, such eccentrically viewed targets have been shown to increase the variability of fixation eye movements,30 and may have had a similar impact on pursuit eye velocity.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…A lower accuracy would suggest that the target was not being continuously foveated. Indeed, this would be consistent with the results of Shanidze et al,14 who found that small targets are not always foveated during pursuit. In typical observers, such eccentrically viewed targets have been shown to increase the variability of fixation eye movements,30 and may have had a similar impact on pursuit eye velocity.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…Pursuit eye movements are likely to be no exception. For instance, in one of the few studies of the 2D nature of smooth pursuit, Shanidze et al14 sampled eye position as observers tracked a target that moved through the eight cardinal directions. The subsequent analyses quantified the proximity of eye position samples (60 Hz) to the pursuit target, and the results demonstrated that, for all pursuit directions, eye position varied not just along the target trajectory, but also orthogonal to it.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Central vision loss results in deficits in visual functions such as acuity [4], contrast sensitivity [5], colour discrimination [6,7], insensitivity to flicker [8,9], delayed dark adaptation [10], shape perception [11], face recognition [12,13], stereopsis [14] and reading [15,16]. Patients adapt to the loss of central vision with the habitual use of an area of eccentric retina called the preferred retinal locus (PRL) as the new point of reference for the ocular motor system [17,18,19] Research on the ocular motor consequences of central vision loss has included fixation stability [20,21], saccades [22,23,24], smooth pursuit [25,26,27,28] and optokinetic nystagmus [29]. The vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) of patients with AMD and other low vision patients with various aetiologies has been explored along with other predictors of the functional success of telescopic spectacle use [30,31].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Strabismus is one of the eye movement disorders, and confirming of version eye movement, in which smooth pursuit eye movement (SPEM), 1-8 in nine directions is important to detect limitation of eye movement. 9-11 In most ophthalmology clinics, the examiner visually evaluates SPEM.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%