2005
DOI: 10.1121/1.1944647
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Acoustical cues for sound localization by the Mongolian gerbil, Meriones unguiculatus

Abstract: The present study measured the head-related transfer functions (HRTFs) of the Mongolian gerbil for various sound-source directions, and explored acoustical cues for sound localization that could be available to the animals. The HRTF exhibited spectral notches for frequencies above 25 kHz. The notch frequency varied systematically with source direction, and thereby characterized the source directions well. The frequency dependence of the acoustical axis, the direction for which the HRTF amplitude was maximal, w… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

8
82
1

Year Published

2007
2007
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 71 publications
(91 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
8
82
1
Order By: Relevance
“…We trained gerbils to place their nose in a ring to disrupt a light beam and elicit a sound from one of two speakers, which were spaced symmetrically about the midline at various angles. The sounds were low-frequency noise bursts (Ͻ1.2 kHz) for which ITD is the only cue available for localization (Maki and Furukawa, 2005). The ability of individual gerbils to correctly identify the speaker that emitted the sound for a range of separation angles is shown in Figure 1b.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…We trained gerbils to place their nose in a ring to disrupt a light beam and elicit a sound from one of two speakers, which were spaced symmetrically about the midline at various angles. The sounds were low-frequency noise bursts (Ͻ1.2 kHz) for which ITD is the only cue available for localization (Maki and Furukawa, 2005). The ability of individual gerbils to correctly identify the speaker that emitted the sound for a range of separation angles is shown in Figure 1b.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The ability of individual gerbils to correctly identify the speaker that emitted the sound for a range of separation angles is shown in Figure 1b. Individual threshold separation angles ranged from 7°to 20°, corresponding to a difference in ITD of ϳ11-30 s, with a median value of 14°, or 21 s (approximate ITDs were derived from the head-related transfer functions in Maki and Furukawa (2005)). These values are comparable to the threshold difference for ITD in humans (Mills, 1958).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Thus, for reference ITDs of 0-200 μs most neurons are responding on the slope, where discrimination is best, while for a reference ITD of 300 μs most neurons are forced to detect changes near their peaks, where discrimination is less optimal. It would be interesting to test the ITD discrimination in a species with a head even smaller than the rabbit, such as a gerbil, where the physiological window is expected to be about 120 μs (Maki and Furukawa, 2005). Because the ITD tuning of neurons is similar between rabbits and gerbils (Spitzer and Semple, 1995) this might be a case where the decrease in ITD discrimination does not match the physiological window.…”
Section: Comparison With Neural Thresholdsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…3B), causing a plateau in the summed firing rate at contralateral azimuths. Since the ITD range of gerbils (Maki and Furukawa, 2005) is about one-half that of rabbits (Kim et al, 2010;Day et al, 2012) or cats (Tollin and Koka, 2009), and one-fifth that of humans, the success of the single-channel decoder in gerbils (at low frequencies) is therefore likely due to their small ITD range.…”
Section: Comparison To Previous Sound Localization Decoding Studiesmentioning
confidence: 99%