2002
DOI: 10.3758/bf03194941
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Adapting a memory framework (source monitoring) to the study of closure processes

Abstract: The present experiments adapt a memory framework (source monitoring) to the study of closure processes. Closure processes are invoked as explanatory mechanisms underlying the ability to identify objects under conditions of incomplete visual information. If closure processes are activated, filling in missing pieces of visual information, intriguing memory predictions follow. When making source judgments about the way in which visual information was experienced initially (e.g., complete or incomplete in form), a… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2002
2002
2008
2008

Publication Types

Select...
3

Relationship

1
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 40 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Compensation afforded by closure on healthy controls was partial with Frag-15% and Frag-25% figures, but was total for Frag-0%, thus suggesting that representations of Frag-0% figures were that of the figures in their complete version. Foley, Foley, Durso, and Smith (1997) and Foley, Foley, and Korenman (2002) provided support to this possibility by showing that healthy participants, when asked to decide the version (complete or incomplete) in which figures were presented previously, tend to claim that incomplete figures were presented in a complete fashion. In opposition to this idea of total compensation, one may postulate that Frag-0% figures were simply not closed because they had not to be closed.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 82%
“…Compensation afforded by closure on healthy controls was partial with Frag-15% and Frag-25% figures, but was total for Frag-0%, thus suggesting that representations of Frag-0% figures were that of the figures in their complete version. Foley, Foley, Durso, and Smith (1997) and Foley, Foley, and Korenman (2002) provided support to this possibility by showing that healthy participants, when asked to decide the version (complete or incomplete) in which figures were presented previously, tend to claim that incomplete figures were presented in a complete fashion. In opposition to this idea of total compensation, one may postulate that Frag-0% figures were simply not closed because they had not to be closed.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 82%
“…More generally, the absence of an advantage for letter transformation tasks leading to nonwords may have less to do with the unimportance of cognitive operations information per se and more to do with the possibility that memory tests vary in their sensitivity to the presence of cognitive operations information. Consistent with this point, the wording of source judgments has important consequences for accuracy (e.g., Foley et al, 2002;R. L. Marsh & Hicks, 1998).…”
mentioning
confidence: 70%
“…or reading (e.g., "did you read this item or not?"). Within the source-monitoring framework, variations in the wording of source tests are expected to influence accuracy by affecting the characteristics of memory traces that are consulted when rendering these judgments (e.g., Foley et al, 2002;Johnson et al, 1993;Johnson et al, 1981; R. L. Marsh & Hicks, 1998).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations