1994
DOI: 10.1006/ceps.1994.1022
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Advice and Feedback: Elements of Practice for Problem Solving

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

1
38
0
3

Year Published

2007
2007
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 57 publications
(42 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
1
38
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…In an experiment that tested feedback specificity and its relationship to learning, Phye and Sanders (1994) tested two types of feedback (i.e., general advice versus specific feedback, the latter providing the learner with the correct answer). Students were assigned to one of the two learning conditions, and they received either general advice or specific feedback as part of a verbal analogy problem-solving task.…”
Section: Feedback Specificitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In an experiment that tested feedback specificity and its relationship to learning, Phye and Sanders (1994) tested two types of feedback (i.e., general advice versus specific feedback, the latter providing the learner with the correct answer). Students were assigned to one of the two learning conditions, and they received either general advice or specific feedback as part of a verbal analogy problem-solving task.…”
Section: Feedback Specificitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While this technique is established as a means of obtaining rich data on users' cognitive processes (e.g., Chi et al 2008), surprisingly it is not often applied in the context of ITS evaluation (although exceptions exist, e.g., D'Mello et al 2006;Muldner and Conati 2010). We should point out that there already is substantial work on the utility of help, both in the cognitive science and the ITS communities (e.g., Phye and Sanders 1994;Chi et al 2008;Hays et al 2009;Razzaq and Heffernan 2009). However, there is a lack of agreement across studies as to which type of feedback is best, as is pointed out by various meta-analyses (e.g., Shute 2008).…”
Section: Discussion and Future Workmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some researchers suggest that bottom-out hints can be useful for learning (Shih et al 2008), but do not compare the utility of various types of hints. Work directly comparing various levels of feedback found that specific feedback, i.e., providing the answer, is better than general high-level advice (Phye and Sanders 1994). In contrast, however, there is also evidence that high-level scaffolding, which is at least somewhat comparable to general hints, fosters learning better than simply providing didactic explanations (e.g., the answer) (Chi et al 2008;Chi 2010).…”
Section: Discussion and Future Workmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In case of low prior knowledge students, the process of building cognitive schemas may have been slowed down because new information interfered with the previous information that had not been organized and automated well enough. The linkage of the new information might have been problematic in this situation (Phye and Sanders 1994).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%