2000
DOI: 10.1080/036107300750015714
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Age Differences in On-Line Syntactic Processing

Abstract: Reading time and comprehension for subject-relative (e.g., The pilot that admired the nurse dominated the conversation) and object-relative (e.g., The pilot that the nurse admired dominated the conversation) constructions were compared among younger and older readers. Younger adults, but not older adults, differentially allocated time to the more taxing object-relative constructions. Although there were no age differences in comprehension of subject-relative constructions, older adults demonstrated lower level… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

3
22
1
2

Year Published

2006
2006
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 68 publications
(28 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
3
22
1
2
Order By: Relevance
“…If so, then our finding that the inferences are understood and remembered to the same degree for older adults as young adults contradicts conclusions from previous studies that older adults' memory for propositions is poorer (e.g., Cohen, 1979; Engelkamp & Zimmer, 1997; Kemper, 1987; Kemtes & Kemper, 1997; Light & Capps, 1986; Meyer & Rice, 1981; Stine & Wingfield, 1988, 1990; Stine-Morrow, Ryan, & Leonard, 2000; Zurif, Swinney, Prather, Wingfield, & Brownell, 1995). However, again, these studies compared performance only on empirical measures, RTs and accuracy, not the individual components of processing that give rise to those measures.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 90%
“…If so, then our finding that the inferences are understood and remembered to the same degree for older adults as young adults contradicts conclusions from previous studies that older adults' memory for propositions is poorer (e.g., Cohen, 1979; Engelkamp & Zimmer, 1997; Kemper, 1987; Kemtes & Kemper, 1997; Light & Capps, 1986; Meyer & Rice, 1981; Stine & Wingfield, 1988, 1990; Stine-Morrow, Ryan, & Leonard, 2000; Zurif, Swinney, Prather, Wingfield, & Brownell, 1995). However, again, these studies compared performance only on empirical measures, RTs and accuracy, not the individual components of processing that give rise to those measures.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 90%
“…On the other hand, we recognized that there is evidence that the mechanisms that support performance in STM tasks play a role in aspects of what may be called an “expanded” comprehension process. In many studies, end-of-sentence performance has been significantly correlated with WM capacity (King and Just, 1991; MacDonald et al, 1992; Just and Carpenter, 1992; Stine-Morrow et al, 2000; Kemper and Kemtes, 1999; see Waters and Caplan, 1996a and Caplan and Waters, 1999, for additional references). To account for this, Caplan and Waters (1999) argued that the postulated specialized memory system supported the initial, preferred, discourse congruent interpretation of a sentence (what we called “interpretive processing”), and that revisions of this structure and interpretation and the use of the product of the comprehension process to perform a task (what we called “post-interpretive processing”) could make use of mechanisms that support performance in STM tasks.…”
Section: The Boy Who the Girl Who Fell Down The Stairs Grabbed Losmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, age differences in sentence comprehension accuracy are larger for sentences that are more semantically or syntactically complex, and these differences in performance have been found to be dependent upon individual differences in verbal WM capacity ( Kemper, 1987 ; Stine and Hindman, 1994 ; Stine-Morrow et al, 2000 ; Christianson et al, 2006 ; Payne et al, 2014a ). For example, “garden path” sentences such as (1) introduce a temporary syntactic ambiguity.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Typically, the first verb warned is initially (and incorrectly) interpreted as the main verb of the sentence (rather than as the verb of the reduced relative clause), creating difficulty when the reader encounters the second verb conducted ; resolution thus requires a revision of the initial analysis ( Bever, 1970 ; Clifton et al, 2003 ), which entails maintaining the alternate parse of the sentence during processing. WM capacity is an important predictor of resolution in garden-path ambiguities in younger ( Just and Carpenter, 1992 ; MacDonald et al, 1992 ; Just and Varma, 2002 ) and older ( Kemtes and Kemper, 1997 ; Kemper et al, 2004 ; Christianson et al, 2006 ) adults, as well as in other syntactically complex constructions, such as object-relative clauses ( Stine-Morrow et al, 2000 ; DeDe et al, 2004 ), and long distance dependencies ( King and Kutas, 1995 ; Caplan et al, 2011 ; Payne et al, 2014a ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%