Previous studies have shown that amygdala lesions impair avoidance of an electrified probe. This finding has been interpreted as indicating that amygdala lesions reduce fear. It is unclear, however, whether amygdala-lesioned rats learn that the probe is associated with shock. If the lesions prevent the formation of this association, then pretraining reversible inactivation of the amygdala should impair both acquisition and retention performance. To test this hypothesis, the amygdala was inactivated (tetrodotoxin; TTX; 1 ng/side) before a shock-probe acquisition session, and retention was tested 4 d later. The data indicated that, compared with rats infused with vehicle, rats infused with TTX received more shocks during the acquisition session, but more importantly, were not impaired on the retention test. In Experiment 2, we assessed whether the spared memory on the retention test was caused by overtraining during acquisition. We used the same procedure as in Experiment 1, with the exception that the number of shocks the rats received during the acquisition session was limited to four. Again the data indicated that amygdala inactivation did not impair performance on the retention test. These results indicate that amygdala inactivation does not prevent the formation of an association between the shock and the probe and that shock-probe deficits during acquisition likely reflect the amygdala's involvement in other processes.The ability to learn and remember the relationships between aversive events and the stimuli that predict them is an adaptive capacity essential to the survival of animals. Fear conditioning, a paradigm in which the subject learns that an initially neutral stimulus is associated with a fear-eliciting stimulus or event, is typically used to study the neural mechanisms of learned fear. Extensive evidence indicates that the amygdala is involved in the learning and memory of conditioned fear. For instance, amygdala lesions may induce anterograde and retrograde amnesia for instrumental and Pavlovian fear conditioning (Liang et al. 1982;Dunn and Everitt 1988;Phillips and LeDoux 1992;Sananes and Davis 1992;Kim and Davis 1993;Parent et al. 1995a;LaBar and LeDoux 1996;Lee et al. 1996;Maren et al. 1996;Bermudez-Rattoni et al. 1997;Muller et al. 1997;Maren 1998Maren , 1999Poremba and Gabriel 1999;Wilensky et al. 1999;Antoniadis and McDonald 2001).In contrast, other evidence indicates that the amygdala is involved in, but not necessary for the retention of conditioned fear (Vanderwolf et al. 1988;Sutherland and McDonald 1990;Helmstetter 1992;Parent et al. 1992Parent et al. , 1995bHelmstetter and Bellgowan 1994;Killcross et al. 1997;Cahill et al. 2000;Lehmann et al. 2000). For example, we recently demonstrated that the amygdala does not appear to be necessary for retrograde memory for cued fear conditioning (Lehmann et al. 2000). The findings showed that rats given amygdala lesions 4 d after being exposed to an electrified probe did not show any deficits when memory was tested after the induction of the lesi...