1988
DOI: 10.3758/bf03214196
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Apparent rotation in three-dimensional space: Effects of temporal, spatial, and structural factors

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
52
1

Year Published

1990
1990
2012
2012

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 75 publications
(56 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
3
52
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The results demonstrated that while there are significant age-related differences in performance, human observers' shape discrimination ability is tolerant of large disruptions in surface coherence: in particular, both older and younger observers could reliably discriminate surface shape when fewer than half of the stimulus points fell on coherent surfaces (and when the majority of the stimulus points thus served as masking "volumetric noise"). Because human observers effectively perceive 3-D shape from motion in addition to binocular disparity (e.g., Andersen, 1996;Braunstein, 1966;Domini, Caudek, & Richman, 1998;Norman & Lappin, 1992;Todd, Akerstrom, Reichel, & Hayes, 1988), Experiment 2 was designed to similarly evaluate the effects of reductions in surface coherence for 3-D surfaces defined by motion.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The results demonstrated that while there are significant age-related differences in performance, human observers' shape discrimination ability is tolerant of large disruptions in surface coherence: in particular, both older and younger observers could reliably discriminate surface shape when fewer than half of the stimulus points fell on coherent surfaces (and when the majority of the stimulus points thus served as masking "volumetric noise"). Because human observers effectively perceive 3-D shape from motion in addition to binocular disparity (e.g., Andersen, 1996;Braunstein, 1966;Domini, Caudek, & Richman, 1998;Norman & Lappin, 1992;Todd, Akerstrom, Reichel, & Hayes, 1988), Experiment 2 was designed to similarly evaluate the effects of reductions in surface coherence for 3-D surfaces defined by motion.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One might question how a quantitative study of SFM interpolation can be meaningful when Todd and his collaborators (Todd, Akerstrom, Reichel, & Hayes, 1988;Todd & Bressan, 1990;Todd & Norman, 1991) have provided evidence that depth is recovered only up to an affine transformatiort in SFM displays. Rescaling of depth along the line of sight, however, would not necessarily affect the results that we obtained with our SFM probe.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, the computational theorem that three distinct views of four noncoplanar points are both necessary and sufficient for obtaining a unique interpretation of an object's 3-D structure (formulated by Ullman, 1979) has been successfully exploited in several psychophysical studies (e.g., Braunstein, Hoffman, & Pollick, 1990;Braunstein, Hoffman, Shapiro, Andersen, & Bennett, 1987;Domini, Caudek, & Prottitt, 1997;Doner, Lappin, & Perfetto, 1984;Lappin, Doner, & Kottas, 1980;Todd, Akerstrom, Reichel, & Hayes, 1988;Todd & Bressan, 1990). The general conclusion from this research is that the ability of human observers to perceive structure from motion is sometimes more powerful than expected on the basis of some computational models (see also Todd, 1985).…”
Section: Human Performance Versus Mathematical Constraintsmentioning
confidence: 99%