2020
DOI: 10.1007/s10071-020-01446-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Arthropod spatial cognition

Abstract: The feats of arthropods, and of the well-studied insects and crustaceans in particular, have fascinated scientists and laymen alike for centuries. Arthropods show a diverse repertoire of cognitive feats, of often unexpected sophistication. Despite their smaller brains and resulting lower neuronal capacity, the cognitive abilities of arthropods are comparable to, or may even exceed, those of vertebrates, depending on the species compared. Miniature brains often provide parsimonious but smart solutions for compl… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
5

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 61 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…A well-established literature extends the concepts of consciousness—the capability of having phenomenal experiences, however basic or minimally structured—and cognition to phylogenetically basal systems, including free-living or facultatively communal unicells, whether pro- or eukaryotic ( Maturana and Varela 1980 ; Pattee 1982 ; Stewart 1996 ; di Primio et al 2000 ; Lyon 2015 ; Baluška and Levin 2016 ; Baluška and Reber 2019 , 2021 ; Levin 2019 , 2020 ; Fields and Levin 2020a ; Lyon 2020 ; Baluška et al 2021 ), plants ( Calvo and Kiejzer 2009 ; Gagliano and Grimonprez 2015 ; Baluška et al 2018 ; Debono and Souza 2019 ; Calvo et al 2020 ), and aneural or lower (than mammals, birds, or cephalopods) complexity neural metazoa, particularly flatworms ( Shomrat and Levin 2013 ; Inoue et al 2015 ; Levin 2020 ) and insects ( Menzel 2012 ; Perry et al 2017 ; Pfeffer and Wolf 2020 ). Like the extension of these concepts from humans to nonhuman mammals and then to big-brained nonmammals, this extension to more basal organisms was initially motivated by observations of communication, learning, and behavioral flexibility, and by functional similarities between the mechanisms supporting information processing and learning in basal systems and in more complex systems such as mammals.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A well-established literature extends the concepts of consciousness—the capability of having phenomenal experiences, however basic or minimally structured—and cognition to phylogenetically basal systems, including free-living or facultatively communal unicells, whether pro- or eukaryotic ( Maturana and Varela 1980 ; Pattee 1982 ; Stewart 1996 ; di Primio et al 2000 ; Lyon 2015 ; Baluška and Levin 2016 ; Baluška and Reber 2019 , 2021 ; Levin 2019 , 2020 ; Fields and Levin 2020a ; Lyon 2020 ; Baluška et al 2021 ), plants ( Calvo and Kiejzer 2009 ; Gagliano and Grimonprez 2015 ; Baluška et al 2018 ; Debono and Souza 2019 ; Calvo et al 2020 ), and aneural or lower (than mammals, birds, or cephalopods) complexity neural metazoa, particularly flatworms ( Shomrat and Levin 2013 ; Inoue et al 2015 ; Levin 2020 ) and insects ( Menzel 2012 ; Perry et al 2017 ; Pfeffer and Wolf 2020 ). Like the extension of these concepts from humans to nonhuman mammals and then to big-brained nonmammals, this extension to more basal organisms was initially motivated by observations of communication, learning, and behavioral flexibility, and by functional similarities between the mechanisms supporting information processing and learning in basal systems and in more complex systems such as mammals.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It should be noted that the aforementioned cognitive abilities and intelligence are not limited to cephalopods only. In fact, there are some evidence supporting that arthropods, particularly bees and ants, also have behavioral flexibility and advanced cognitive abilities 41 , 42 .…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The cognitive requirements of the various mechanisms of orientation differ. Yet, many of the seemingly complex mechanisms have been documented among organisms that were previously thought to lack such abilities (i.e., invertebrates), and the previous decades have yielded explosive growth in studies evaluating orientation in these organisms (see references in Gaffin & Curry, 2020; Ortega‐Escobar, 2020; Pfeffer & Wolf, 2020; Warrant & Dacke, 2010). For example, the parasitic wasp Hyposoter hotiola uses visual landmarks to track host eggs and find potential plants that may contain those eggs (Van Nouhuys & Kaartinen, 2008).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These may be coupled with navigational cues (compass directions) with all systems aligned to create a navigational system, which allows the ant to maintain a trajectory toward a specific goal (Freas & Spetch, 2019; Pfeffer & Whittlinger, 2016; Wehner, 2003; Wehner et al., 2016). Even complex navigational abilities utilizing a map and compass sense can be found among arthropods (Pfeffer & Wolf, 2020). For example, dung beetles ( Scarabaeus lamarcki ) use a celestial compass as a cue to guide their navigations indicating map‐based navigation (Dacke et al., 2014).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%