2001
DOI: 10.3758/bf03194539
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Asymmetrical contrast effects induced by luminance and color configurations

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
32
0

Year Published

2003
2003
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(36 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
4
32
0
Order By: Relevance
“…His intuition that distinct object borders influence our perception of color and contrast is consistent with studies showing interactions between color appearance and the spatial profile of surface contours, or the geometric configuration of the visual display (Devinck et al, 2006;De Weert and Spillmann, 1995;Dresp and Fischer, 2001;Fach and Sharpe, 1986;Pinna, 2008Pinna, , 2011Pinna and Reeves, 2006).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 55%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…His intuition that distinct object borders influence our perception of color and contrast is consistent with studies showing interactions between color appearance and the spatial profile of surface contours, or the geometric configuration of the visual display (Devinck et al, 2006;De Weert and Spillmann, 1995;Dresp and Fischer, 2001;Fach and Sharpe, 1986;Pinna, 2008Pinna, , 2011Pinna and Reeves, 2006).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 55%
“…He therein defined what later has become known as simultaneous color contrast (e.g. Beck, 1966;De Weert, 1984;De Weert and Spillmann, 1995;Dresp and Fischer, 2001;Gerrits and Vendrik, 1970;Heinemann, 1955;Helson, 1963;Pinna, 2008;Shapley and Reid, 1985) or color context effects (e.g. Long and Purves, 2003;Reeves et al, 2008;Shevell and Kingdom, 2008).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The contrast polarity of the red stimuli was varied to test for possible functional asymmetries between bright and dark in the perception of ''near'' in colored figures (e.g., Dresp & Fischer, 2001) with varying luminance contrast. Previously, Dresp et al (2002) have shown that ''near'' percepts in pairs of achromatic figures were not affected by whether a dark figure overlapped a bright one, or a bright figure overlapped a dark one.…”
Section: The Contribution Of Color Stereopsismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A measurement of 12 cd/m 2 was found to be close enough to individual points of psychophysical isoluminance for the three colors. Psychophysical isoluminance had been assessed beforehand by two separate procedures consisting of a classic flicker test on the one hand and of minimally distinct border adjustments on the other, as in Dresp and Fischer (2001). In the present experimental display, psychophysical isoluminance varied between 11.82 cd/m 2 and 12.54 cd/m 2 for green, and between 12.22 cd/m 2 and 13.01 cd/m 2 for red when testing for green/red isoluminance.…”
Section: Stimulimentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, it has been shown (Reid & Shapley, 1988) that local contrast is very important in brightness perception, since it determines brightness induction-that is, it makes an object appear brighter on a background of lower luminance and darker on a background of greater luminance. Similarly, Dresp and Fischer (2001) showed that contrast, rather than contrast polarity, is the important factor in the integration of inducing stimuli in a Kaniza's square configuration to produce perceptual filling in. On the contrary, we have shown the contrast has only a limited role in the spatial interaction between noncollinear edges.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%