2022
DOI: 10.1152/jn.00547.2021
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Asymmetrical transfer of adaptation between reaching and tracking: implications for feedforward and feedback processes

Abstract: Reaching and manual tracking are two very common tasks for studying human sensorimotor processes. Although these motor tasks rely both on feedforward and feedback processes, emphasis is more on feedforward processes for reaching, and more on feedback processes for tracking. The extent to which feedforward and feedback processes are interrelated when being updated is not settled yet. Here, using reaching and tracking as proxies, we examined the bidirectional relationship between the update of feedforward and fe… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

2023
2023
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Therefore, participants had to use a continuous sensory feedback to adapt to the statistical fluctuations we imposed for each trial (Clark, 2016;Körding & Wolpert, 2004;Piray & Daw, 2020). The responses we characterized in our task could not be observed by chance, and AR and v T influenced successful attacks and escapes (Coudiere et al, 2022;Domenici et al, 2011;Hocherman & Levy, 2000;Lam & Zenon, 2021). Higher AR and v T values in chasing trials increased uncertainty in the escaping path, making it more challenging for users to reach the target.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Therefore, participants had to use a continuous sensory feedback to adapt to the statistical fluctuations we imposed for each trial (Clark, 2016;Körding & Wolpert, 2004;Piray & Daw, 2020). The responses we characterized in our task could not be observed by chance, and AR and v T influenced successful attacks and escapes (Coudiere et al, 2022;Domenici et al, 2011;Hocherman & Levy, 2000;Lam & Zenon, 2021). Higher AR and v T values in chasing trials increased uncertainty in the escaping path, making it more challenging for users to reach the target.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Second, when visuomotor adaptation is required, the use of continuous tasks minimizes the implication of cognitive strategies as time pressure inherent to these tasks imposes fast deliberation to ongoing actions (Yang et al, 2021;Yoo et al, 2021). Third, asymmetrical transfer of visuomotor adaptation between discrete and continuous tasks confirms that these types of movement are driven by (at least partly) distinct neural mechanisms (Abeele & Bock, 2003;Coudiere et al, 2022;Ikegami et al, 2010). The overarching goal of the current study is to help filling this gap by assessing interlimb differences and transfer in visuomotor adaptation during manual tracking, a task in which a hand dominance effect at baseline (i.e.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Understanding how humans adapt hand movements when confronted to a visuomotor perturbation has relied primarily on discrete reaching movements performed towards a stationary target (Carroll et al, 2014;Henriques & Cressman, 2012;Krakauer et al, 2000;Krakauer, 2009;Seidler, 2006;Wang & Sainburg, 2006). Although considerable knowledge has been brought by these studies, this body of research does not necessarily account for the adaptation of continuous movements (Cohen et al, 2019;Coudiere et al, 2022). First, the contribution of online visual feedback control (i.e.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations