1981
DOI: 10.1037/0021-843x.90.5.468
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Attribution and alcohol-mediated aggression.

Abstract: Forty-eight male subjects participated in an aggression task after having received either alcoholic or placebo beverages and after being told that they had received either a high dose or low dose of alcohol. All subjects in the alcohol group actually received the same dose. Measures of aggression were the intensity and duration of shock given to a bogus partner in a reaction time-pain perception task and a score on a posttask attitude questionnaire. Blood alcohol readings were taken three times during the sess… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

1986
1986
2007
2007

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 41 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 25 publications
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…If the researcher also wishes to determine the properties of alcohol consumption that impact aggression-that is, determine if the effect is pharmacological, psychological, or both-then this design is inadequate; comparing participants in the alcohol group with those in the control confounds pharmacological effects with expectancy effects 2 (Bushman, 1997). To overcome this problem, some researchers adopted a balanced-placebo design in which alcohol consumption and alcohol expectancy were fully crossed (see Gustafson, 1985a;Pihl, Zeichner, Niaura, Nagy, & Zacchia, 1981). As shown in Fig.…”
Section: Experimental Studies Of Alcohol and Aggressionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…If the researcher also wishes to determine the properties of alcohol consumption that impact aggression-that is, determine if the effect is pharmacological, psychological, or both-then this design is inadequate; comparing participants in the alcohol group with those in the control confounds pharmacological effects with expectancy effects 2 (Bushman, 1997). To overcome this problem, some researchers adopted a balanced-placebo design in which alcohol consumption and alcohol expectancy were fully crossed (see Gustafson, 1985a;Pihl, Zeichner, Niaura, Nagy, & Zacchia, 1981). As shown in Fig.…”
Section: Experimental Studies Of Alcohol and Aggressionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, intoxicated subjects rated a partner with evil intent as more aggressive. Pihl et al [120] found increased aggression after alcohol intoxication but the effect of expectations was not evaluated. It was not possible to evaluate the disinhibition hypothesis in these studies because the existence of inhibition of aggression was not measured.…”
Section: Rating Scalesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The beverage and the sample strongly influence the result in different studies [119][120][121]. Gustafson [122] found no increased aggression as expressed by Cezarek and Marke Personality System (CMPS) after beer or wine consumption.…”
Section: Disinhibition Of Aggressive Behaviormentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Most of the research on alcohol-related aggression has typically compared an alcohol group with a placebo and/ or a knowingly sober control group. Only 2 studies found an equal increase in aggression for persons given alcohol and those given a placebo (Lang et al, 1975;Pihl et al, 1981), and 1 investigation showed that aggression actually decreased for persons given an alcoholic beverage relative to those given a placebo (Rohsenow and Bachorowski, 1984). In contrast, 5 meta-analytic investigations demonstrated that whereas alcohol groups display significantly greater levels of aggression compared with placebo and sober control groups, placebo and sober controls do not tend to differ significantly (Bushman, 1997;Bushman and Cooper, 1990;Hull and Bond, 1986;Ito et al, 1996;Steele and Southwick, 1985).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%