This article examines a basic question concerning the nature of episodic associations. On the one hand, associations can be viewed as directional pointers connecting different mental representations. While studying a pair of words in a memory experiment (A-B), subjects strengthen a pointer going from A to B and a separate one going from B to A, thus forming two distinct unidirectional associations. This view maps onto the classic independent association hypothesis (IAH), which viewed forward and backward associations as distinct, separately modifiable (e.g., Ebbinghaus,1885Ebbinghaus, /1913Robinson, 1932), and even statistically independent (Wolford, 1971).Arguing against the IAH, Gestalt theorists promoted a principle of associative symmetry (Asch & Ebenholtz, 1962;Köhler, 1947). This alternate view saw an episodically formed association is a holistic conjunction of the A and B representations, without any directional A®B or B®A pointers. According to this associative symmetry hypothesis (ASH), each member of the associated pair can recover the entire pattern, independent of the order of presentation (Asch, 1968;Asch & Ebenholtz, 1962;Rock & Ceraso, 1964). Experimental evidence on the equality of forward and backward associative recall, discussed below, and the arguments in favor of one-trial associative learning (e.g., Estes, 1960;Rock, 1957) lent support to this position.With the ascent of computational and distributed memory models, this basic question concerning the nature of episodic associations gained renewed importance. Distributed memory models make explicit assumptions about the mechanisms of association. As we will show in this article, some models assume symmetrical associative learning of item representations, whereas other models allow for separate formation of forward and backward associations.Studies aimed at discriminating the IAH from the ASH examined the dependence of recall on the temporal order of encoding.This was examined by using the classic pairedassociate method. Subjects studied randomly paired meaningful items, denoted A-B, in temporal succession. Then the experimenter tested each pair by either probing with A for recall of B (a forward test) or probing with B for recall of A (a backward test). A finding of asymmetric retrieval (e.g., better forward than backward recall) was seen as evidence against ASH and in favor of IAH. Similarly, a failure to find the expectedasymmetry result was taken as support for the ASH, at least on grounds of parsimony (e.g., Asch & Ebenholtz, 1962;Murdock, 1962).Even if forward and backward recall are equivalent on average, this does not strictly imply symmetry. Rather, the ASH requires that for every studied pair, A-B, the probability of recalling B given A perfectly predicts the probability of recalling A given B. That is, the correlation between forward and backward recall, at the level of individual pairs, must be 1.0. At the other extreme, the IAH (e.g., Wolford, 1971) implies a correlation of 0.0 between forward and backward recall.Although there ha...