2018
DOI: 10.1093/biolinnean/bly119
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Being “hangry”: food depletion and its cascading effects on social behaviour

Abstract: Evolutionary theory suggests two alternative ways in which competitive interactions could vary in response to different levels of food abundance. Competition theory suggests that aggression should be greater when resource availability is lower, as an evolutionary stable strategy to access food. Alternatively, energy allocated to aggressive interactions should increase when the available spectrum of food resources is wider, in turn allowing a greater selec-tion. We tested these hypotheses on a group-living herb… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

1
21
1

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 138 publications
(173 reference statements)
1
21
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Overall, our findings seem to militate against an effect of interference on vigilance. This is in apparent contrast to what has been reported for many gregarious species in which social monitoring of competitors triggers alert behaviour (birds: Ely, ; McKinstry & Knight, ; Goss‐Custard et al, ; mammals: Favreau et al, ; Fattorini, Brunetti et al, ; Pecorella et al, ). The difference could be related to the small number of individuals in roe deer groups (maximum six individuals; Supporting Information Figure S1): few competitors may be tolerated, if the increase in collective vigilance within a group provides individual feeding benefits (cf.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 63%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Overall, our findings seem to militate against an effect of interference on vigilance. This is in apparent contrast to what has been reported for many gregarious species in which social monitoring of competitors triggers alert behaviour (birds: Ely, ; McKinstry & Knight, ; Goss‐Custard et al, ; mammals: Favreau et al, ; Fattorini, Brunetti et al, ; Pecorella et al, ). The difference could be related to the small number of individuals in roe deer groups (maximum six individuals; Supporting Information Figure S1): few competitors may be tolerated, if the increase in collective vigilance within a group provides individual feeding benefits (cf.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 63%
“…Accordingly, we found a decrease in both time in vigilance and vigilance rate—along with an increase in time spent foraging—with increasing group size of roe deer. Either an absence or a reversal of the group‐size effect is expected in individuals belonging to sex/age classes involved in agonistic interactions, for example because of feeding/mating interference (birds: McKinstry & Knight, ; Goss‐Custard et al, ; mammals: Fattorini, Brunetti et al, ; Pecorella, Fattorini, Macchi, & Ferretti, ). Unlike females (but see Roviani, ), roe deer males are territorial from late winter to mid‐summer and show aggressive behaviours to conspecifics (reviewed in Liberg et al, ); dominance hierarchies have been reported in winter, through aggressive interactions within roe deer groups (Espmark, ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In our study, we found that animals in CU3 showed the highest aggression (66.59%), followed by CU1 (43.64%) and CU2 (37.75%). We assumed that the higher aggression in CU3 was due to the presence of other species [52]. Punjab urial males were frequently observed chasing chinkara, showing comparatively less aggression toward mouflon sheep.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Alternatively, the energy allocated to aggressive interactions could increase with the value of a disputed resource (Enquist et al, 1985;Geist, 1978;Parker, 1974). Accordingly, individuals could invest more in body growth (e.g.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First, because of the mating system and reproductive seasonality in migratory guanaco populations (Franklin, 1982(Franklin, , 1983Young & Franklin, 2004a), we expected that the probability and frequency of aggressive interactions between male guanacos would peak during the mating period (Prediction I) and would be higher in males from family groups because of the behaviour's reproductive significance (Prediction II). Second, because resource-defence ungulates fight for territories which contain attractive food resources for females (Emlen & Oring, 1977;Franklin, 1983;Young & Franklin, 2004a, 2004b, we expected that the presence and the frequency of aggressive interactions in territorial males (family groups and solitary males) would be higher in areas of greater forage quality and productivity (Prediction III), given that territorial males can invest more energy in aggressive interactions where resources are higher (Enquist et al, 1985;Geist, 1978;Parker, 1974) and that they also have priority access to females and mating. Finally, because non-territorial males (bachelors and mixed groups) do not engage in reproduction, we expect that aggression in these social units will be linked to food resources (Fattorini, et al, 2018;Sirot, 2000) and will be higher in areas of lower forage quality and productivity due to increased competition (Prediction IV).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%