1980
DOI: 10.1177/014616728063023
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Better Liked than Right

Abstract: A persuasive message on the subject of international maritime boundaries was presented in pamphlet form to 1055 students in four countries. Trustworthiness and expertise of the source were manipulated in a 2 x 2 x 4factorial design of the after-only type to assess the relative impact of each component on the communicator's persuasiveness. Main effects were found for both country and trustworthiness. Overall, the expert and trustworthy source generated the most opinion change. However, the trustworthy communica… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
63
0
3

Year Published

2000
2000
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 359 publications
(72 citation statements)
references
References 6 publications
6
63
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Thus, in contrast to the differences in source evaluations of usefulness and trustworthiness (Appel & Malečkar, 2012), introducing the story to be fiction did not reduce the persuasive effect. This result is remarkable, as higher source trustworthiness has been associated with higher persuasion in previous research on nonnarrative persuasion (e.g., McGinnies & Ward, 1980;see Pornpitakpan, 2004, for an overview). Appel and Malečkar (2012) suggest that the persuasion parity of nonfiction and fiction is due to the subordinate role of information that accompanies a story (paratexts, Genette, 1987) as compared to the story itself in narrative persuasion.…”
Section: On the Trustworthiness Of Fictional And Nonfictional Informasupporting
confidence: 52%
“…Thus, in contrast to the differences in source evaluations of usefulness and trustworthiness (Appel & Malečkar, 2012), introducing the story to be fiction did not reduce the persuasive effect. This result is remarkable, as higher source trustworthiness has been associated with higher persuasion in previous research on nonnarrative persuasion (e.g., McGinnies & Ward, 1980;see Pornpitakpan, 2004, for an overview). Appel and Malečkar (2012) suggest that the persuasion parity of nonfiction and fiction is due to the subordinate role of information that accompanies a story (paratexts, Genette, 1987) as compared to the story itself in narrative persuasion.…”
Section: On the Trustworthiness Of Fictional And Nonfictional Informasupporting
confidence: 52%
“…Subsequent research established that credibility is not merely one concept, but is constituted of several components including trustworthiness (Hovland, Janis, & Kelley, 1953), expertise (McGinnies & Ward, 1980), goodwill (Walthen & Burkell, 2002), enterprise/experience (Pornpitakpan, 2004), and authority and character (McCroskey, 1966). In reviewing 50 years of credibility research, Pornpitakpan (2004) point out that the main effects of nearly all studies suggest that high credibility sources are more effective in causing attitude change and behaviors desired by the communicator than are low credibility sources.…”
Section: Credibilitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Whether or not we take advice from someone is likely to be influenced not only by our attitudes towards specific advice-givers and the social groups that they represent (e.g., female, Asian), but also attitudes about their trustworthiness (McGinnies & Ward, 1980;Van't Wout & Sanfey, 2008), perceived competence (McGinnies & Ward, 1980;Todorov, Mandisodza, Goren, & Hall, 2005), friendliness (Oehler & Kohlert, 2009), and similarity to "us" (Burger, Messian, Patel, del Prado, & Anderson, 2004;Gino, Shang, & Croson, 2009). Prejudice, then, manifested as the unwarranted preference for taking advice from members of one social group over another, may reflect one-or a number of-these beliefs.…”
Section: Experiments 4: Beliefs Underlying Decisions On the Advice Taskmentioning
confidence: 99%