2018
DOI: 10.31234/osf.io/3sc9j
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Brain-outcome associations for risk taking depend on the measures used to capture individual differences

Abstract: Biological markers of risk taking are prominent targets for clinical, developmental, and longitudinal research. With respect to brain function, several regions are considered central for risky choice, yet insights into the neural basis of risk taking stem primarily from studies using single measures. Considering that recent studies suggested different risk-taking measures cannot be used interchangeably, it is currently unclear whether core regions of the brain involved in risk show a measure-dependent function… Show more

Help me understand this report
View published versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
12
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

3
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 68 publications
(207 reference statements)
2
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Concerning the latter, our anterior insula findings are consistent with AIM predictions for age e↵ects on neural correlates of (risky) decision making [27]; relative to younger adults, older adults showed decreased activation in brain regions associated with loss anticipation, specifically the anterior insula. Confirming the crucial role of behavioral measures in brain-behavior associations [85], and by extension age e↵ects, we obtained mainly insignificant specifications for neural markers extracted from delay discounting. We did obtain a small set (n=3) of significant specifications indicating positive age e↵ects on nucleus accumbens activation di↵erences for smaller-sooner versus larger-later choices, but these were highly covariate-dependent and as such less credible than the other age e↵ects obtained from our analyses.…”
Section: Multiverse Analysis: Specification Curvessupporting
confidence: 65%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Concerning the latter, our anterior insula findings are consistent with AIM predictions for age e↵ects on neural correlates of (risky) decision making [27]; relative to younger adults, older adults showed decreased activation in brain regions associated with loss anticipation, specifically the anterior insula. Confirming the crucial role of behavioral measures in brain-behavior associations [85], and by extension age e↵ects, we obtained mainly insignificant specifications for neural markers extracted from delay discounting. We did obtain a small set (n=3) of significant specifications indicating positive age e↵ects on nucleus accumbens activation di↵erences for smaller-sooner versus larger-later choices, but these were highly covariate-dependent and as such less credible than the other age e↵ects obtained from our analyses.…”
Section: Multiverse Analysis: Specification Curvessupporting
confidence: 65%
“…In the current study we aimed to address these shortcomings and performed a multiverse analysis [84] of life span trajectories of risk preference and related constructs using multiple traditional and novel elicitation methods. The current study presents a follow-up and extension of our previous work [25,31,43,46,85], combining comprehensive within-subject testing of di↵erent constructs (i.e., risk preference, impulsivity, and low self-control) and assessment methods (i.e., self report, informant report, behavioral paradigms, and biological measures) in an adult life span sample spanning from early to late adulthood. The two main questions of interest were thus: (1) Do we find age e↵ects on risk preference, impulsivity and low self-control, and do these converge given these constructs conceptual relatedness?, and (2) To what extent do age e↵ects for risk preference, impulsivity, and low self-control converge as a function of assessment method?…”
Section: Current Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Such improvements are much needed in various areas of behavioral research (cf. Frey et al, 2017;Lauriola et al, 2014;Lönnqvist et al, 2015;Millroth, Juslin, Winman, Nilsson, & Lindskog, 2020)-for instance, when investigating the functional neural architecture of risk taking in neuroimaging studies (e.g., Schonberg et al, 2012Schonberg et al, , 2011Tisdall et al, 2018).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…), yet previous research has highlighted substantial psychometric limitations therein (e.g., Beauchamp, Cesarini, & Johannesson, 2017;Berg, Dickhaut, & Mc-Cabe, 2005;Eisenberg et al, 2019;Frey, Pedroni, Mata, Rieskamp, & Hertwig, 2017;Lönnqvist, Verkasalo, Walkowitz, & Wichardt, 2015;Slovic, 1962). Although valid alternatives do exist in the form of self-report measures (e.g., Steiner, Seitz, & Frey, 2020), behavioral tasks may be indispensable for applications such as studying the functional neural architecture of risk preference, which requires the simulation of risk-taking behaviors in the fMRI scanner (e.g., Helfinstein et al, 2014;Li et al, 2019;Rao, Korczykowski, Pluta, Hoang, & Detre, 2008;Schonberg, Fox, & Poldrack, 2011;Tisdall et al, 2018).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation