2016
DOI: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1187116
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Brooding rumination and attentional biases in currently non-depressed individuals: an eye-tracking study

Abstract: Both rumination and attentional biases have been proposed as key components of the RDoC Negative Valence Systems construct of Loss. Although theorists have proposed that rumination, particularly brooding rumination, should be associated with increased sustained attention to depression-relevant information, it is not clear whether this link would be observed in a non-depressed sample or whether it is specific to brooding versus reflective rumination. To address these questions, the current study examined the li… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

5
35
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 45 publications
(40 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
5
35
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Consistent with Experiment 1, a retraction was effective in the high-DR group for negative misinformation only. This finding may be attributed to the salience of the negative misinformation, based on an attentional bias towards negative information in depressive ruminators (E. P. Chang et al, 2017;Koster, De Raedt, Leyman, & De Lissnyder, 2010;Owens & Gibb, 2017). This is in line with the notion that misinformation that is more salient at the time of its correction is more easily retracted (Ecker et al, 2017), potentially due to enhanced coactivation (Kendeou et al, 2014) and/or conflict detection (Stadtler et al, 2013; also see Ecker et al, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 52%
“…Consistent with Experiment 1, a retraction was effective in the high-DR group for negative misinformation only. This finding may be attributed to the salience of the negative misinformation, based on an attentional bias towards negative information in depressive ruminators (E. P. Chang et al, 2017;Koster, De Raedt, Leyman, & De Lissnyder, 2010;Owens & Gibb, 2017). This is in line with the notion that misinformation that is more salient at the time of its correction is more easily retracted (Ecker et al, 2017), potentially due to enhanced coactivation (Kendeou et al, 2014) and/or conflict detection (Stadtler et al, 2013; also see Ecker et al, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 52%
“…Studies using the dot-probe task have demonstrated an association between the use of habitual rumination (Donaldson, Lam, & Mathews, 2007) and particularly brooding (Joormann et al, 2006;Southworth, Grafton, MacLeod, & Watkins, 2017) with larger attention biases for negative words at 1,000-ms stimuli presentations. Similarly, eye-tracking studies using free-viewing paradigms have shown that habitual brooders are characterized by both more sustained attention to sad faces and less sustained attention to happy faces (Duque, Sanchez, & Vazquez, 2014;Owens & Gibb, 2017), a double bias that is commonly observed in depressed individuals (see Armstrong & Olatunji, 2012). Such associations in these studies were independent of concurrent levels of depression, suggesting that they may reflect factors of vulnerability rather than consequences of current depression states.…”
Section: External Attentional Disengagement and Broodingmentioning
confidence: 58%
“…Similar to Owens and Gibb (2017) and Krejtz et al (2018), we measured attentional bias by calculating gaze duration for specific facial expressions controlling for total gaze duration for all facial expressions. We used the relative score not an absolute gaze duration because it is a more direct measure of an attentional bias reflecting the difference between processing of happy, sad, angry, and neutral faces, and it is currently the standard procedure in studying attentional biases.…”
Section: Eye-tracking Measures Of Focus On Emotional Facial Expressionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We used the relative score not an absolute gaze duration because it is a more direct measure of an attentional bias reflecting the difference between processing of happy, sad, angry, and neutral faces, and it is currently the standard procedure in studying attentional biases. We calculated relative scores for each of the facial expressions using a standard formula (e.g., Everaert et al 2014;Owens and Gibb 2017). The total gaze duration for each type of face (target face in the formula below) was divided by the total viewing time for all facial expressions.…”
Section: Eye-tracking Measures Of Focus On Emotional Facial Expressionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation