2020
DOI: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2019.108534
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Capturing the dynamics of peripersonal space by integrating expectancy effects and sound propagation properties

Abstract: Highlights• Measuring expectation helps to interpret audiotactile integration behavioral impact • Tactile RTs follow a logarithmic decrease due to audiotactile integration • Logarithmically distributed auditory distances provide more pertinent RTs measures • PPS is better characterized and quantified with these enhancements Abstract BackgroundHumans perceive near space and far space differently. Peripersonal space (PPS), i.e. the space directly surrounding the body, is often studied using paradigms based on au… Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
17
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
0
17
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The complete lack of an expectancy effect ( Kandula et al. 2017 ; Hobeika et al. 2019 ) is not as unusual for the visuo-tactile pairing as it is for the audio-tactile pairing (e.g., Pfeiffer et al.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 98%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The complete lack of an expectancy effect ( Kandula et al. 2017 ; Hobeika et al. 2019 ) is not as unusual for the visuo-tactile pairing as it is for the audio-tactile pairing (e.g., Pfeiffer et al.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…These trials are fundamental in ascertaining that putative effects in the experimental trials are veritably due to multisensory interactions, and not simply due to an expectancy effect (which would be time-dependent and thus revealed in the baseline trials; see Kandula et al. 2017 and Hobeika et al. 2019 ).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In this behavioral paradigm, however, participants are instructed not to respond to visual stimuli alone. This experimental design choice was driven by the fact that inclusion of “catch, non-response” trials greatly reduces the expectation bias that results from having to respond on every trial (see Kandula et al, 2017 and Hobeika et al, 2019 for more detail). However, not indexing unisensory visual-only trials impedes from ascertaining whether true multisensory integration occurred, via race (Miller, 1982) or driftdiffusion (Drugowitsch et al, 2014) models, both requiring RTs to every component (e.g., V, T, and VT).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…At odds with some previous studies employing auditory or visual looming stimuli (Canzoneri et al, 2012;Finisguerra et al, 2015;Noel et al, 2015;Serino et al, 2015), we used 'static' stimuli briefly and synchronously flashed with tactile ones to avoid the risk of inflating the estimates of multisensory facilitation. In fact, looming stimuli with predictable arrival times induce foreperiod effects that, though not solely responsible for the boosting of touch, may lead to overestimations of the magnitude of the facilitation (Hobeika et al, 2020;Holmes et al, 2020;Kandula et al, 2017).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%