2010
DOI: 10.1007/s00221-010-2421-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Catching a gently thrown ball

Abstract: Several studies have shown that people can catch a ball even if it is visible only during part of its flight. Here, we examine how well they can do so. We measured the movements of a ball and of the hands of both the thrower and the catcher during one-handed underarm throwing and catching. The catcher's sight was occluded for 250 ms at random moments. Participants could catch most balls without fumbling. They only really had difficulties if vision was occluded before the ball was released and was restored less… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
45
1

Year Published

2012
2012
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 46 publications
(52 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
6
45
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The estimated individual delays ranged from 0.193 and 0.217 s for all the subjects. As these figures can be interpreted as the time between the moment when the optical information crosses a threshold and the initiation of action, the reported values are consistent with sensorio-motor delays estimated in similar tasks [26].…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 86%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The estimated individual delays ranged from 0.193 and 0.217 s for all the subjects. As these figures can be interpreted as the time between the moment when the optical information crosses a threshold and the initiation of action, the reported values are consistent with sensorio-motor delays estimated in similar tasks [26].…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 86%
“…The fact that people move more rapidly when intercepting fast objects [25] suggest an interplay between sensory and motor phases with possible overlapping periods of sensory information acquisition and motor action. The longer one sees a ball's trajectory the lower the error in predicting future positions but always leaving room to perform the action [26]. How the total time is allocated for perception and action reflects knowledge of sensory and movement uncertainty [27].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…An apparent exception is a study of catching by López-Moliner, Brenner, Louw, and Smeets (2010), in which balls were repeatedly tossed from a distance of 75 cm, with a mean flight time of 483 ms, and randomly occluded for 250ms. Occlusion did not affect the “quality” of the catch, as long as the ball's motion was visible at release or 400-200 ms before the catch.…”
Section: Model-based Control: Is An Internal Model Sufficient?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A mapping is a quantitative correlation between optical variables and action variables or goal-states that can be used to guide behavior. As an example, recall López-Moliner et al's (2010) observation that, when tossing a ball back and forth, random occlusion did not affect catching if the ball was visible at release. The authors conclude that catching is normally continuously controlled, but can be guided by the ball's initial motion if the trajectory is occluded.…”
Section: Anticipatory Control Of Interceptive Action: Is An Internmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Nevertheless parabolic trajectories have been objects of attention in several studies that have examined how catching performance depends on vision of different parts of the parabolic path (e.g., Sharp and Whiting, 1974, 1975; Whiting and Sharp, 1974; Dessing et al, 2009; López-Moliner et al, 2010) or which visual information in fly balls is used to predict the landing point (Chapman, 1968; Oudejans et al, 1997; McLeod et al, 2003; Brouwer et al, 2006; Fink et al, 2009). However, putting forward a computational model that specifies how humans can time the parabolic catch has remained elusive.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%