2019
DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13325
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children’s Intuitive Theories of Academic Performance

Abstract: This is the author manuscript accepted for publication and has undergone full peer review but has not been through the copyediting, typesetting, pagination and proofreading process, which may lead to differences between this version and the Version of Record. Please cite this article as

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
36
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(38 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
2
36
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Even for a child who themselves believes that ability is malleable, praise for being "smart" (or other forms of person-focused praise) might communicate that their evaluator highly values successful performance rather than effort or learning. From this, the child may infer that, in the presence of that evaluator, they need to hide their failures and appear to effortlessly succeed; indeed, even young children recognize that people seem smarter when they succeed without effort (Muradoglu & Cimpian, 2019). In private, however, they might continue to work hard and take on challenges.…”
Section: Reputation and Praisementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Even for a child who themselves believes that ability is malleable, praise for being "smart" (or other forms of person-focused praise) might communicate that their evaluator highly values successful performance rather than effort or learning. From this, the child may infer that, in the presence of that evaluator, they need to hide their failures and appear to effortlessly succeed; indeed, even young children recognize that people seem smarter when they succeed without effort (Muradoglu & Cimpian, 2019). In private, however, they might continue to work hard and take on challenges.…”
Section: Reputation and Praisementioning
confidence: 99%
“…As children pick up on these subtle cues, they might strive to attain feedback from others that makes them seem smart relative to their peers by strategically approaching or avoiding evaluators who tend to praise selectively . Inferences about their ability relative to others may even shape children's reputation management earlier in development; as early as age 5, children infer that an individual who expends little effort to achieve a successful outcome is smarter than someone who works hard to achieve the same outcome (Muradoglu & Cimpian, 2019). Additionally, by this age, children cheat more often in a game after overhearing an experimenter say that a classmate is smart (Zhao et al, 2019).…”
Section: What We Can Learn About Reputation From the Achievement Literaturementioning
confidence: 99%
“…As an exploratory analysis inspired by Muradoglu and Cimpian (2020), the aforementioned logistic regression model was used to estimate predicted probabilities at 0.1-year increments. This analysis allowed us to approximate the age at which children's responses were significantly above chance.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Such computations are subserved by metacognitive insight into one’s performance and this could be a critical factor in deciding to engage in effortful tasks (Dunn & Risko, 2016). Indeed, younger children have been found to conflate effort exerted with their ability (Muradoglu & Cimpian, 2020; Nicholls, 1978), and metacognitive abilities have been reported to improve with age (Chevalier & Blaye, 2016; Shin et al, 2007). This could account for potential developmental changes in effort-related decision-making, particularly when these are made in the context of implicit tasks.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%