1988
DOI: 10.1016/0010-0285(88)90017-5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

38
964
5
11

Year Published

2001
2001
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 1,059 publications
(1,018 citation statements)
references
References 12 publications
38
964
5
11
Order By: Relevance
“…It is as if children are astounded at the very idea that a language would have terms for the hues of things. Many related results appear in the literature of vocabulary acquisition: Young children take shape as a more salient cue than size or color (Landau, Smith & Jones, 1998), they favor wholes over parts (Markman & Wachtel, 1988), objects over their component properties (Hall, Waxman & Hurwitz, 1993), and so forth. In none of these cases can it be argued that the children don't "see in color," "discriminate sizes," etc.…”
Section: Summary and Discussion Of The Findingsmentioning
confidence: 77%
“…It is as if children are astounded at the very idea that a language would have terms for the hues of things. Many related results appear in the literature of vocabulary acquisition: Young children take shape as a more salient cue than size or color (Landau, Smith & Jones, 1998), they favor wholes over parts (Markman & Wachtel, 1988), objects over their component properties (Hall, Waxman & Hurwitz, 1993), and so forth. In none of these cases can it be argued that the children don't "see in color," "discriminate sizes," etc.…”
Section: Summary and Discussion Of The Findingsmentioning
confidence: 77%
“…It is predicted that performance on this task will approach ceiling, as typical in the literature (Haryu, 1991;Markman & Wachtel, 1988). In this and subsequent experiments we continue to employ a single False Belief task in view of the number of other measures and the young age of participants.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 89%
“…This tendency can be demonstrated experimentally using what is known as the disambiguation paradigm (e.g., Markman & Wachtel, 1988;Merriman & Bowman, 1989). In the presence of a familiar nameable object and an unfamiliar object children are asked to pick the referent of a novel name.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…These alternative accounts make quite distinct predictions about the generality of children's responses in Markman and Wachtel's (1988) task. According to a lexical-constraints account, children's choice of an object without a name in response to a novel label was caused by their honoring a principle specific to word learning (the mutual exclusivity bias or the novel-name nameless-category principle) and therefore should occur only in word-learning contexts.…”
Section: Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Children chose the object without a name (e.g., the tongs) approximately 80% of the time (see also Bowman, 1989, andBertrand, 1994, for comparable results). Markman and Wachtel (1988) explained these findings by arguing that children adhered to a mutual exclusivity bias. That is, children assumed that an object could not have more than one label.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%