2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.11.008
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Color categories and color appearance

Abstract: We examined categorical effects in color appearance in two tasks, which in part differed in the extent to which color naming was explicitly required for the response. In one, we measured the effects of color differences on perceptual grouping for hues that spanned the blue–green boundary, to test whether chromatic differences across the boundary were perceptually exaggerated. This task did not require overt judgments of the perceived colors, and the tendency to group showed only a weak and inconsistent categor… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
58
0
1

Year Published

2013
2013
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
2

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 54 publications
(62 citation statements)
references
References 62 publications
(106 reference statements)
3
58
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…A second potential nonlinearity that has been a focus of several studies could be in the degree of categorical coding, such that observers give more weight to the dominant hue component when judging the proportions (Regier & Kay, 2009). Following Webster and Kay (Webster & Kay, 2012), we modeled this as a relative weighting of the pure categorical response [the unique hue angle of the dominant component (Θ c )], and the linear response (Θ l ): Θpred=bΘnormalc+(1normalb)Θnormall Figures 4i and 4j show the predictions for a standard deviation of 0.15 in the value for the bias (b) [somewhat weaker than the the biases estimated by Webster and Kay (2012) for the Malkoc et al (2005) hue-scaling functions]. As with the response nonlinearity, the bias systematically alters all hues between the unique and binary axes and thus again leads to a broad pattern of loadings.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A second potential nonlinearity that has been a focus of several studies could be in the degree of categorical coding, such that observers give more weight to the dominant hue component when judging the proportions (Regier & Kay, 2009). Following Webster and Kay (Webster & Kay, 2012), we modeled this as a relative weighting of the pure categorical response [the unique hue angle of the dominant component (Θ c )], and the linear response (Θ l ): Θpred=bΘnormalc+(1normalb)Θnormall Figures 4i and 4j show the predictions for a standard deviation of 0.15 in the value for the bias (b) [somewhat weaker than the the biases estimated by Webster and Kay (2012) for the Malkoc et al (2005) hue-scaling functions]. As with the response nonlinearity, the bias systematically alters all hues between the unique and binary axes and thus again leads to a broad pattern of loadings.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…With regard to scaling, a conceptual issue is to what extent hue scaling itself is an actual measure of appearance, for it could be argued that the task is a variant on color naming where the observers are judging the salience of a small set of categories. Moreover, these judgments show some susceptibility to categorical biases with regard to the primaries used for scaling, and these biases could reflect an intrusion of linguistic coding (Webster & Kay, 2012). Nevertheless, responses from hue-scaling experiments reveal spectral sensitivities that closely resemble the functions measured by more nonverbal tasks such as hue cancellation [e.g.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…in the judged similarity or the time required to discriminate a pair of colors) (Franklin, Drivonikou, Bevis, Davies, Kay & Regier, 2008, Gilbert, Regier, Kay & Ivry, 2006, Mullen & Kulikowski, 1990, Winawer, Witthoft, Frank, Wu, Wade & Boroditsky, 2007). However, these categorical effects tend to be subtle and labile (Brown, Lindsey & Guckes, 2011, Witzel & Gegenfurtner, 2011, Witzel & Gegenfurtner, 2013), and may depend strongly on whether the task used to measure them is limited by the properties of perceptual encoding versus the decision stages of the response or language processing (Kay & Kempton, 1984, Pilling, Wiggett, Ozgen & Davies, 2003, Roberson & Davidoff, 2000, Roberson, Pak & Hanley, 2008, Webster & Kay, 2012). Thus it remains unknown to what extent differences in naming patterns are tied to differences in perception.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…What counts as uniformity will therefore differ between observers. Moreover, one may expect color-specific response effects with large sets of colors because spanning linguistically or categorically separable regions is known to influence discrimination thresholds and response times [7]. These effects seem to originate in perception.…”
Section: Stimulus-specific Features Of Perceptionmentioning
confidence: 98%