2000
DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.129.3.369
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Color categories are not universal: Replications and new evidence from a stone-age culture.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

15
445
1
5

Year Published

2002
2002
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 497 publications
(466 citation statements)
references
References 91 publications
(167 reference statements)
15
445
1
5
Order By: Relevance
“…Recently, however, an opposed view has also gained prominence, arguing that color naming varies far more across languages than had been suspected (e.g. Lucy, 1997;Roberson, Davies, & Davidoff, 2000)-and that even languages with similar color naming systems differ in the placement of boundaries between categories (Roberson, Davidoff, Davies, & Shapiro, 2005). Overall, we have an empirically mixed picture, with both universal tendencies in color naming and some deviation from those tendencies (Regier, Kay, & Khetarpal, 2007)-mirroring the general "cluster and outlier" pattern found when considering other aspects of language in cross-language perspective (Evans & Levinson, 2009: 445).…”
Section: Case Study 1: Color a Continuous Domainmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recently, however, an opposed view has also gained prominence, arguing that color naming varies far more across languages than had been suspected (e.g. Lucy, 1997;Roberson, Davies, & Davidoff, 2000)-and that even languages with similar color naming systems differ in the placement of boundaries between categories (Roberson, Davidoff, Davies, & Shapiro, 2005). Overall, we have an empirically mixed picture, with both universal tendencies in color naming and some deviation from those tendencies (Regier, Kay, & Khetarpal, 2007)-mirroring the general "cluster and outlier" pattern found when considering other aspects of language in cross-language perspective (Evans & Levinson, 2009: 445).…”
Section: Case Study 1: Color a Continuous Domainmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…English speakers by Roberson, Davies & Davidoff (2000). Note that the higher the score, the poorer the fit.…”
Section: Color Categories 38mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, within these diverse naming systems there are noticeable generalities (Kay, Berlin & Merrifield, 1991;MacLaury, 1987) It is the finding of such generalities that led to the proposal of panhuman universals in cognitive color categorization that transcend terminological differences (e.g., Heider & Olivier, 1972). Roberson, Davies & Davidoff (2000) reported a series of experiments that set out to replicate and extend the work of Rosch Heider in the early 1970s (Rosch Heider, 1972, Heider & Olivier, 1972. Rosch Heider's experiments had been particularly influential in promoting the view that language and cognitive experience are largely independent (in some cases, orthogonal).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…According to prototype models, the color space is divided into (more or less) universal categories anchored around particular focal hues (Berlin & Kay, 1969;Kay & McDaniel, 1978;MacLaury, 1991;Rosch, 1975;Rosch Heider, 1972; but see Roberson, Davies, & Davidoff, 2000). For example, there tends to be greater consensus about the best example of blue (i.e., focal blue) than about the boundary between blue and purple.…”
Section: Similarity-based Models Of Categorizationmentioning
confidence: 99%