1992
DOI: 10.1016/s1057-7408(08)80043-5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Comparative Advertising and Brand Evaluation: Toward Developing a Categorization Approach

Abstract: A categorization model was proposed to account for the effects of direct, indirect, and no comparison advertising on aspects of new brand evaluation. Direct comparisons were hypothesized to promote exemplar‐based processing, and indirect comparisons were hypothesized to promote prototype‐based processing. Evaluation of attributes of the new brand were hypothesized to depend on the type of processing promoted by the comparative ads. Subjects used comparative response scales to evaluate attributes of the new bra… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

2
28
0

Year Published

1993
1993
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(30 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
2
28
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This finding is consistent with previous studies showing that relative measures (subjective measures requiring consumer judgment about similarities or differences between brands) are more sensitive to comparative advertising effects than are nonrelative measures (objective measures of performance, devoid of a comparison; Grewal et al, 1997;Miniard et al, 1993Miniard et al, , 1998Pechmann and Ratneshwar, 1991;Snyder, 1992). However, it should be noted that these findings were not replicated in the video conditions, as we found no evidence that named versus unnamed comparisons differentially affected perceptions there.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…This finding is consistent with previous studies showing that relative measures (subjective measures requiring consumer judgment about similarities or differences between brands) are more sensitive to comparative advertising effects than are nonrelative measures (objective measures of performance, devoid of a comparison; Grewal et al, 1997;Miniard et al, 1993Miniard et al, , 1998Pechmann and Ratneshwar, 1991;Snyder, 1992). However, it should be noted that these findings were not replicated in the video conditions, as we found no evidence that named versus unnamed comparisons differentially affected perceptions there.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…Nonetheless, these studies have not provided an unambiguous test of Hypothesis 1 because of measurement confounds. For example, although the comparative and noncomparative ads examined in Snyder's (1992) study produced similar responses to nonrelative attitude measures, persuasion differences favoring the comparative ads were uncovered by Downloaded by [Deakin University Library] at 15:34 15 March 2015 differentiative relative measures of attribute beliefs. However, because type of measure is confounded with type of construct, one cannot attribute unambiguously the observed effects to only type of measurement.…”
Section: Research Hypothesis and Existing Evidencementioning
confidence: 89%
“…However, because type of measure is confounded with type of construct, one cannot attribute unambiguously the observed effects to only type of measurement. Thus, Snyder's (1992) findings may be indicating either that attribute belief measures are more responsive than attitude measures or that relative measures are more responsive than nonrelative measures. This limitation reoccurs throughout the literature.…”
Section: Research Hypothesis and Existing Evidencementioning
confidence: 95%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Consumers also make comparisons in or among advertisements (Zhang, Kardes, & Cronley, 2002). Some advertisements invoke comparisons because they explicitly contrast products (Snyder, 1992). Others invoke comparisons because the target ad is simply seen alongside competing products (Malaviya, Kisielius, & Sternthal, 1996).…”
Section: The Role Of Relative Comparison In Attribute Preferencementioning
confidence: 99%