Handbook of Research Methods in Experimental Psychology 2003
DOI: 10.1002/9780470756973.ch10
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Comparative Psychology

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
12
0

Year Published

2007
2007
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 71 publications
0
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We no longer live in a scientific world of monolithic theoretical institutions, but rather one featuring a web of interconnected, specialist subfields. Papini ( 2011 , p. 211) beautifully captured this idea in saying: “As a field, comparative psychology is almost interdisciplinary by definition…[and] demands not only knowledge of psychological theories and techniques, but an understanding of behavioral neuroscience, comparative neurology, behavioral ecology, developmental biology, and evolutionary theory.” This echoes the suggestion of Shettleworth ( 2009 , p. 216) that “exploiting and nourishing those connections, and teaching our students to do the same, is an essential part of the future of the field.” The answer that thus seems obvious to us is that the next generation of comparative psychologists will come from the same constituent fields as the experts that comprise or complement the discipline, in perhaps only a slightly more skewed manner than they already always have.…”
Section: What's In a Name?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We no longer live in a scientific world of monolithic theoretical institutions, but rather one featuring a web of interconnected, specialist subfields. Papini ( 2011 , p. 211) beautifully captured this idea in saying: “As a field, comparative psychology is almost interdisciplinary by definition…[and] demands not only knowledge of psychological theories and techniques, but an understanding of behavioral neuroscience, comparative neurology, behavioral ecology, developmental biology, and evolutionary theory.” This echoes the suggestion of Shettleworth ( 2009 , p. 216) that “exploiting and nourishing those connections, and teaching our students to do the same, is an essential part of the future of the field.” The answer that thus seems obvious to us is that the next generation of comparative psychologists will come from the same constituent fields as the experts that comprise or complement the discipline, in perhaps only a slightly more skewed manner than they already always have.…”
Section: What's In a Name?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As learning and memory organization appear to have emerged in a long distant evolutionary past, they utilize vital and deeply rooted neural mechanisms and extend across broad phylogenetic divisions (Giurfa & Sandoz, 2012;Menzel & Benjamin, 2013). Despite the phenomena's broad taxonomic distribution, much of our knowledge regarding learning comes from only a small number of vertebrate species (Mackintosh, 1974;Papini, 2008). Unfortunately, the neural complexities associated with the vertebrate brain present significant challenges for research into the biological mechanisms of learning, and argue for the inclusion of alternate, more accessible model organisms.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Animal cognition research covers a wide range of topics, from how animals learn and remember to how they make decisions and how they interact with other individuals. By studying a wide number of questions in an equally wide range of species, the field broadly aims to understand the mechanisms, functions and the evolution of cognition (although exact definitions depended on context, for example see Beran et al, (2014), Olmstead & Kuhlmeier, (2015) and Shettleworth (2009) for 'comparative cognition' and for 'comparative psychology' see Call et al, (2017) and Papini, (2003)). However, studying animal minds-that are in principle unobservable-is challenging (Allen & Bekoff, 1999;Stevens, 2010), and the process is shaped by a variety of assumptions about minds, animals, and knowledge (Andrews & Monsó, 2021), as well as the history of the field itself.…”
Section: The Hidden Side Of Animal Cognition Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%