1962
DOI: 10.1037/h0048046
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Concept identification as a function of obviousness of relevant and irrelevant information.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

2
37
0

Year Published

1969
1969
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 60 publications
(39 citation statements)
references
References 4 publications
2
37
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, using flower patterns as stimuli, Trabasso (1963) found that identification of the relevant dimension, stem-leaves angle, was faster when the angle was colored red than when the color of the angle was the same as the rest of the flower. Similarly, Archer (1962) found Psychon. Sci., 1969, Vol.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, using flower patterns as stimuli, Trabasso (1963) found that identification of the relevant dimension, stem-leaves angle, was faster when the angle was colored red than when the color of the angle was the same as the rest of the flower. Similarly, Archer (1962) found Psychon. Sci., 1969, Vol.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Cutting and Schatz (1976) found that in a card-sorting task typicality interacted with proximity of categories, with very long decision times for poor examples of semantically close categories. With respect to the study of CI performance also Archer's (1962) findings on obviousness are of interest. Obviousness was defined as the magnitude of the difference between two categories.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The first aim was to investigate the effect on CI performance of varying the typicality of dimensional values. Following Rosch's (1973) line -of thought on the facilitating effect of prototypical information on learning, and from Archer's (1962) findings regarding obviousness, it was predicted that CI performance would be better when the relevant (or defining) values (R) were typical (typ) than when they are atypical (atyp). Furthermore, when the irrelevant (or nondefining) values (I) were atypical, CI performance would be better than when they were typical.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Observations that the rate of CI is often dependent upon the dimension that is relevant to the solution suggests that the probability of a cue being attended to will determine, in part, the ease of CI when that cue is relevant. Various investigators have postulated attention factors such as discriminability (Archer, Boume, & Brown, 1955), obviousness (Sechrest & Kaas, 1965;and Archer, 1962), and the ratio of the weight of relevant cues to the weight of aß cues (Boume & Restle, 1959; RestIe, 1962;and Bower & Trabasso, 1964).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Observations that the rate of CI is often dependent upon the dimension that is relevant to the solution suggests that the probability of a cue being attended to will determine, in part, the ease of CI when that cue is relevant. Various investigators have postulated attention factors such as discriminability (Archer, Boume, & Brown, 1955), obviousness (Sechrest & Kaas, 1965;and Archer, 1962), and the ratio of the weight of relevant cues to the weight of aß cues (Boume & Restle, 1959; RestIe, 1962;and Bower & Trabasso, 1964).Inherent in these studies is the suggestion that the obviousness or discriminability of the dimensions of the stimuli will determine the preference of Ss for utilizing the dimensions in CI. Imai & Gamer (1965) have attempted to determine the relationship between discriminability and preference in a card-sorting task.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%