The Wiley Blackwell Handbook of Operant and Classical Conditioning 2014
DOI: 10.1002/9781118468135.ch10
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Conditioned Reinforcement

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
10
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 69 publications
0
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Suboptimal choice as evidenced in the procedures summarized above provides a challenge for models of choice that minimize or eliminate the role of conditioned reinforcement (Davison & Baum, ; Shahan, , ; see Bell & McDevitt, ). Other lines of evidence make similar arguments for the concept of conditioned reinforcement as incorporated in the SiGN hypothesis (e.g., Bell & Williams, ; Williams, ).…”
Section: Implications and Future Directionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Suboptimal choice as evidenced in the procedures summarized above provides a challenge for models of choice that minimize or eliminate the role of conditioned reinforcement (Davison & Baum, ; Shahan, , ; see Bell & McDevitt, ). Other lines of evidence make similar arguments for the concept of conditioned reinforcement as incorporated in the SiGN hypothesis (e.g., Bell & Williams, ; Williams, ).…”
Section: Implications and Future Directionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It first should be noted that the terms marking and bridging as used in AAB differ from their technical definitions in the psychology of learning literature. Bell and McDevitt (2014, p. 235; see also Dorey & Cox, 2018; Feng et al, 2016), for example, observed that
[s]uperficially, conditioned reinforcement, marking, and bridging procedures are quite similar: they all allow a connection between an event (stimulus or behavior) and its temporally distant consequence … in conditioned reinforcement procedures the stimulus occurs only after a correct response (i.e., a response leading to primary reinforcement) has been made, whereas with marking and bridging the stimulus occurs regardless of the choice response” (p. 235).
…”
Section: Response Establishment and Maintenancementioning
confidence: 99%
“…The second level of the hierarchy describes what we refer to as contextual values , representing stimuli or actions that initially do not have any inherent value, but that acquire it in virtue of their ability to predict basic values in certain contexts. Again, this is inspired by associative learning literature, and precisely by the concept of conditioned (or secondary) reinforcer (Bell & McDevitt, 2014; Rescorla, 1980; Skinner, 1953). Empirical evidence indicates that, if an initially neutral stimulus such as a token is associated with an unconditioned reinforcer such as food, then animals’ actions resulting in the token will be reinforced, even if these actions never lead to food (Bell & McDevitt, 2014).…”
Section: Theorymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Again, this is inspired by associative learning literature, and precisely by the concept of conditioned (or secondary) reinforcer (Bell & McDevitt, 2014; Rescorla, 1980; Skinner, 1953). Empirical evidence indicates that, if an initially neutral stimulus such as a token is associated with an unconditioned reinforcer such as food, then animals’ actions resulting in the token will be reinforced, even if these actions never lead to food (Bell & McDevitt, 2014). This indicates that the token has become a conditioned reinforcer and has acquired motivational value in it of itself (Rescorla, 1980).…”
Section: Theorymentioning
confidence: 99%