2017
DOI: 10.1037/xlm0000321
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Confidence in forced-choice recognition: What underlies the ratings?

Abstract: Two-alternative forced-choice recognition tests are commonly used to assess recognition accuracy that is uncontaminated by changes in bias. In such tests, participants are asked to endorse the studied item out of 2 presented alternatives. Participants may be further asked to provide confidence judgments for their recognition decisions. It is often assumed that both recognition decisions and confidence judgments in 2-alternative forced-choice recognition tests depend on participants' assessments of a difference… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

0
23
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2025
2025

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
0
23
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Both models have the advantage of reducing costly storage and computation requirements in maintaining the full posterior probability distribution over many unchosen alternatives; in many real-life scenarios, this factor may overcome the need to minimize error in the expected estimation of motion direction, Confidence, or other similar judgments. Additionally, despite reports that memory confidence appears to reflect the balance of evidence at the single neuron level 72 , it has also been suggested that similar Decision-Congruent Evidence dependence may underlie memory confidence in a task specifically designed to compare the two computational approaches 73 , as we did here.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 56%
“…Both models have the advantage of reducing costly storage and computation requirements in maintaining the full posterior probability distribution over many unchosen alternatives; in many real-life scenarios, this factor may overcome the need to minimize error in the expected estimation of motion direction, Confidence, or other similar judgments. Additionally, despite reports that memory confidence appears to reflect the balance of evidence at the single neuron level 72 , it has also been suggested that similar Decision-Congruent Evidence dependence may underlie memory confidence in a task specifically designed to compare the two computational approaches 73 , as we did here.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 56%
“…Mounting evidence points to the existence of metacognitive noise in perceptual decision making (Shekhar & Rahnev, 2021a). Several studies have shown that the addition of metacognitive noise substantially improves model fits (Bang et al, 2019;Maniscalco & Lau, 2016;Shekhar & Rahnev, 2021b;van den Berg et al, 2017), and many others have identified potential sources of this noise (Allen et al, 2016;Fleming et al, 2015Fleming et al, , 2015Maniscalco & Lau, 2016;Peters et al, 2017;Rahnev et al, 2015;Zawadzka et al, 2017). Nevertheless, very little work has been done on understanding the nature of this metacognitive noise with the default assumption of a Gaussian distribution dominating the literature despite not having been directly tested.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, such over-reliance on decision-congruent evidence --i.e., a "confirmation bias" (Abrahamyan et al, 2016) --has also been observed in other post-decisional (non-metacognitive) perceptual judgments (Stocker and Simoncelli, 2008;Luu and Stocker, 2018), value judgments (Brehm, 1956;Festinger, 1957;Gerard and White, 1983;Steele et al, 1993;Heine and Lehman, 1997;Koster et al, 2015), and metamemory (Koriat, 2012;Zawadzka et al, 2016), suggesting it may be a domain-general strategy that serves also to reduce cognitive dissonance and improve self-consistency.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 71%