2010
DOI: 10.1123/jsep.32.3.275
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Conscious Processing and the Process Goal Paradox

Abstract: The three experiments reported here examined the process goal paradox, which has emerged from the literature on goal setting and conscious processing. We predicted that skilled but anxious performers who adopted a global movement focus using holistic process goals would outperform those who used part-oriented process goals. In line with the conscious processing hypothesis, we also predicted that performers using part process goals would experience performance impairment in test compared with baseline condition… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
25
0

Year Published

2011
2011
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 46 publications
(25 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
0
25
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Further, holistic process goals have been found to reduce anxiety (Kingston & Hardy, 1997), which should reduce the likelihood of the monitoring process coming to the fore (Woodman et al, 2015). Moreover, when used by a sample of experienced athletes, holistic process goals such as "reach" and "drive" were associated with superior performance (e.g., less errors) during high-anxiety conditions (Mullen & Hardy, 2010). Thus, we recommend that performers are issued with a clear and simple positive instruction (e.g., holistic process goals), to limit their susceptibility to ironic errors in sport.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Further, holistic process goals have been found to reduce anxiety (Kingston & Hardy, 1997), which should reduce the likelihood of the monitoring process coming to the fore (Woodman et al, 2015). Moreover, when used by a sample of experienced athletes, holistic process goals such as "reach" and "drive" were associated with superior performance (e.g., less errors) during high-anxiety conditions (Mullen & Hardy, 2010). Thus, we recommend that performers are issued with a clear and simple positive instruction (e.g., holistic process goals), to limit their susceptibility to ironic errors in sport.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To help stimulate an effective holistic focus, mood words (e.g., thump, bang, flush, clip, swish) can also provide the athlete with a beneficial "aide memoire" of the whole movement (Rushall, 1979), so long as it accurately corresponds with important movement capacities such as the required strength, speed, power, agility, balance, or endurance (Mullen & Hardy, 2010). As such, the generation, experimentation, and selection of these words may be suitably guided during discussion and video review of successful executions with the sport psychologist.…”
Section: Aim: Internalize the Change To Subconscious Controlmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Interestingly, evidence suggests that many skilled performers remain “somaesthetically” aware (i.e., focusing on the proprioceptive “feel”, Shusterman, 2008) of their movement during on-line skill execution (in the performance context; see Nyberg, in press) and can use global or holistic cue words to improve performance proficiency under pressurized conditions (see Mullen and Hardy, 2010). Therefore, instead of relying wholly on unthinking spontaneity to guide their performance, elite athletes appear to alternate between different modes of cognitive processing, and also between types of bodily self-awareness (i.e., reflective and pre-reflective) in practice and performance contexts.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%