2017
DOI: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1239748
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Context-Specific Adjustment of Cognitive Control: Transfer of Adaptive Control Sets

Abstract: Cognitive control protects processing of relevant information from interference by irrelevant information. The level of this processing selectivity can be flexibly adjusted to different control demands (e.g., frequency of conflict) associated with a certain context, leading to the formation of specific context-control associations. In the present study we investigated the robustness and transferability of the acquired context-control demands to new situations. In three experiments, we used a version of the con… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
22
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2025
2025

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 25 publications
(25 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
3
22
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Taken together, the studies reviewed above provide compelling evidence that specific contextual cues, be they stimulus locations, features or individual items (identities), as well as temporal episodes, can become associated with particular conflict control settings and trigger them in a cue-driven, bottom-up manner. Importantly, though, context-control learning is not limited to conflict-control in Stroop-type tasks, as equivalent effects have also been documented in tasks that probe other components of cognitive control or attention, including task-switching (e.g., Crump and Logan, 2010;Leboe et al, 2008), response inhibition (e.g., Verbruggen and Logan, 2008), dual-tasking (e.g., Fischer et al, 2014;Surrey et al, 2017), Simon task (e.g., Hübner and Mishra, 2016) and attention capture (e.g., Crump et al, 2018). For instance, switch costs -longer response times and lower accuracy when one has to switch rather than to repeat a task set -are thought to reflect control processes of reconfiguring a task-set (Rogers & Monsell, 1995) and/or overcoming interference from a previous set (Allport, Styles, & Hsieh, 1994).…”
Section: Behavioral Evidence For Context-control Learningmentioning
confidence: 97%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Taken together, the studies reviewed above provide compelling evidence that specific contextual cues, be they stimulus locations, features or individual items (identities), as well as temporal episodes, can become associated with particular conflict control settings and trigger them in a cue-driven, bottom-up manner. Importantly, though, context-control learning is not limited to conflict-control in Stroop-type tasks, as equivalent effects have also been documented in tasks that probe other components of cognitive control or attention, including task-switching (e.g., Crump and Logan, 2010;Leboe et al, 2008), response inhibition (e.g., Verbruggen and Logan, 2008), dual-tasking (e.g., Fischer et al, 2014;Surrey et al, 2017), Simon task (e.g., Hübner and Mishra, 2016) and attention capture (e.g., Crump et al, 2018). For instance, switch costs -longer response times and lower accuracy when one has to switch rather than to repeat a task set -are thought to reflect control processes of reconfiguring a task-set (Rogers & Monsell, 1995) and/or overcoming interference from a previous set (Allport, Styles, & Hsieh, 1994).…”
Section: Behavioral Evidence For Context-control Learningmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Dishon-Berkovits and Algom, 2000; Melara and Algom, 2003;Schmidt and Besner, 2008). Specifically, contrary to the assumption of specific context-stimulus-response associations driving this effect, the context-specific performance benefits generalize to new and unbiased stimuli (Bugg, Jacoby, & Chanani, 2011;Crump & Milliken, 2009;Surrey, Dreisbach, & Fischer, 2017). For instance, in the above-mentioned study by Crump & Milliken (2009), the authors created the proportion congruency bias at each location using one subset of stimuli while another subset of stimuli was presented in congruent or incongruent form with equal likelihood at both locations.…”
Section: Behavioral Evidence For Context-control Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, the high CCD associated with the busy overpass may be generalized to the roads near the overpass without directly experiencing high CCD on those roads. Indeed, the generalization of CCD has been documented in human behavior (Crump and Milliken, 2009;King et al, 2012;Weidler and Bugg, 2016;Surrey et al, 2017;Bejjani et al, 2018), although the neurocognitive mechanisms remain poorly understood. As a second goal, we tested the hypothesis that the generalization of CCD can be achieved through integrative encoding (Shohamy and Wagner, 2008;Kuhl et al, 2010), wherein partially overlapping associations (e.g., overpass-road and overpass-CCD) result in the formation of an integrated representation (e.g., overpass-road-CCD) that supports direct retrieval of CCD expectations for an item (e.g., road as cue) that have been inherited from another associated item (e.g., overpass).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Importantly, not all contextual information present in a situation producing conflict can become a cue for future adaptive control. Only certain features of the context become episodically bound (Surrey et al, 2017), and there is evidence that selectively attended features are preferentially encoded. The hippocampus is well known for its role in spatial learning and episodic binding, and its activity during encoding is predictive of subsequent benefits in rapid implicit cueing of attention by contextual features (Goldfarb, Chun, & Phelps, 2016).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%