1997
DOI: 10.1080/02687039708248483
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Contextual influences on comprehension of multiple-meaning words by right hemisphere brain-damaged and non-brain-damaged adults

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2002
2002
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In broad terms, these problems are often described as a consequence of integration deficits, or difficulties constructing a coherent model by assimilating discourse elements into a conceptual whole (see e.g., Tompkins, Fassbinder, Lehman Blake, & Baumgaertner, 2002). Presumed integration deficits are frequently reflected in tasks involving nonliteral forms (e.g., Bihrle, Brownell, & Powelson, 1986;Schmitzer, Strauss, & DeMarco, 1997;Stemmer, Giroux, & Joanette, 1994). More generally, comprehenders with RBD have particular difficulties when required to reconcile multiple, seemingly incongruent inferences or mental models to arrive at a full understanding of a discourse unit.…”
Section: Please Scroll Down For Articlementioning
confidence: 99%
“…In broad terms, these problems are often described as a consequence of integration deficits, or difficulties constructing a coherent model by assimilating discourse elements into a conceptual whole (see e.g., Tompkins, Fassbinder, Lehman Blake, & Baumgaertner, 2002). Presumed integration deficits are frequently reflected in tasks involving nonliteral forms (e.g., Bihrle, Brownell, & Powelson, 1986;Schmitzer, Strauss, & DeMarco, 1997;Stemmer, Giroux, & Joanette, 1994). More generally, comprehenders with RBD have particular difficulties when required to reconcile multiple, seemingly incongruent inferences or mental models to arrive at a full understanding of a discourse unit.…”
Section: Please Scroll Down For Articlementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Second, we specified non-or less biological, cultural, and affective features (Leech, 1974;Brownell et al, 1984;Schmitzer et al, 1997). Cultural (CUL) features were attributes related to human activities (e.g., "is edible").…”
Section: Procedures 1: Selection Of Animals and Featuresmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Subsequent online studies further revealed that RHD patients are impaired in their ability to effectively use context (e.g., Beeman, 1993;Grindrod & Baum, 2003;Klepousniotou & Baum, 2005;Schmitzer et al, 1997;Tompkins et al, 2000; but cf. Leonard & Baum, 1998;Leonard, Baum & Pell, 2001), and coupled with the lack of any time-course effects on their performance, RHD patients seem to be unable to effectively select only the contextually appropriate meaning and eventually suppress inappropriate ones.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Early off-line (i.e., pen and paper) studies (e.g., Brownell, 1988;Brownell, Potter, Michelow, & Gardner, 1984;Brownell, Simpson, Bihrle, Potter, & Gardner, 1990;Schmitzer et al, 1997;Winner & Gardner, 1977) showed that patients with focal RH damage have problems with lexical ambiguity in general, and metaphor in particular. These researchers compared the performance of patients with right hemisphere damage (RHD), left hemisphere damage (LHD) and normal control individuals by using either sentence/context-picture matching 6 (e.g., Schmitzer et al, 1997;Winner & Gardner, 1977) or word triad relatedness judgment (e.g., Brownell, 1988;Brownell, Potter, Michelow, & Gardner, 1984;Brownell, Simpson, Bihrle, Potter, & Gardner, 1990) paradigms. Overall, it was found that when individuals with LHD were presented with ambiguous adjectives (e.g., "warm" to refer to "hot" or "loving"), they chose metaphoric interpretations (e.g., "loving") more frequently and they were less likely to select literal foils (e.g.,…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation