2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.03.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Costly rejection of wrongdoers by infants and children

Abstract: How unappealing are individuals who behave badly towards others? We show here that children and even infants, although motivated by material rewards, are nonetheless willing to incur costs to avoid "doing business" with a wrongdoer. When given the choice to accept a smaller offering from a do-gooder or a larger offering from a wrongdoer, children and infants chose to accept the smaller offering. It was only when the difference between the offerings was very large that their aversion to the wrongdoer was overco… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
49
2

Year Published

2016
2016
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 80 publications
(54 citation statements)
references
References 31 publications
3
49
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Indeed, under SJ , whoever cooperates with an opponent carrying a Bad reputation gets himself a Bad reputation. Interestingly, this judgement is somehow verified in the behaviour of toddlers, who prefer those that mistreat (rather than help) opponents that misbehaved in the past55565758.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 88%
“…Indeed, under SJ , whoever cooperates with an opponent carrying a Bad reputation gets himself a Bad reputation. Interestingly, this judgement is somehow verified in the behaviour of toddlers, who prefer those that mistreat (rather than help) opponents that misbehaved in the past55565758.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 88%
“…In particular, judgments of wrongness develop sensitivity to intentions before judgments of punishment (Cushman et al, 2013). Second, recent work with infants demonstrates that they can take agents' mental states into account when making sociomoral evaluations (Dahl et al, 2013;Hamlin, 2013;Lucca et al, 2018;Tasimi & Wynn, 2016;Woo et al, 2017). Although the nature of the mental states that infants take into account is under debate (Van de Vondervoort & Hamlin, 2016;Woodward, 2009), these results suggest that at least some degree of information about others' mental states can exert an influence on early forms of moral judgment (e.g.…”
Section: The Development Of the Use Of Other's Intentionsmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…By 13 months of age, children use information about distributions a third party makes to others to inform their social evaluations: after observing a third party distribute resources either equally or unequally, infants prefer to interact with (e.g., accept a toy from) someone who distributes fairly (Lucca, Pospisil, & Sommerville, 2018). And, infants give up resources to interact with someone who helps others rather than harms others (Tasimi & Wynn, 2016). At 2 years of age, children prefer to help those who have helped others over those who have harmed others (Dahl, Schuck, & Campos, 2013).…”
Section: The Development Of Punishment and Partner Choice Decisions Imentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although this study focused on who children like, it is worth noting that a number of studies have also investigated who children dislike. Following prior work showing that children reject wrongdoers (Buon et al, 2014;Hamlin, Wynn, & Bloom, 2010;Hamlin et al, 2007;Scola et al, 2015;Tasimi, Johnson, & Wynn, 2017;Tasimi & Wynn, 2016) and dissimilar individuals (Mahajan & Wynn, 2012;Wynn, 2016), it is unclear whether children in our study liked the nice/similar character, disliked the mean/dissimilar character, or both. For example, previous studies report a memory advantage for wrongdoers (Kinzler & Shutts, 2008) and wrongdoings (Baltazar, Shutts, & Kinzler, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 86%