1973
DOI: 10.3758/bf03198064
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Critical issues in interference theory

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

12
266
3
4

Year Published

2000
2000
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
4
4

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 383 publications
(285 citation statements)
references
References 91 publications
12
266
3
4
Order By: Relevance
“…All rearranged pairs were made from forward-going pairs in the study list (i.e., the cue term from pair n was repaired with the response term from pair n ϩ 1, in the case of near re-pairings, or n ϩ 9, in the case of far re-pairings). This was done because previous work suggests that cue words serve as a conceptual peg onto which response terms are bound (Lambert & Paivio, 1956;Paivio, 1991), and thus, if participants were to form associa- Note. Vocabulary scores are from the Shipley (1946) test.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…All rearranged pairs were made from forward-going pairs in the study list (i.e., the cue term from pair n was repaired with the response term from pair n ϩ 1, in the case of near re-pairings, or n ϩ 9, in the case of far re-pairings). This was done because previous work suggests that cue words serve as a conceptual peg onto which response terms are bound (Lambert & Paivio, 1956;Paivio, 1991), and thus, if participants were to form associa- Note. Vocabulary scores are from the Shipley (1946) test.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…High spans may therefore have used controlled attention to inhibit competition from prior list items but were foiled in doing so under retrieval load. Indeed, many theories assume that interference effects reflect a momentary disruption of retrieval (e.g., J. R. Anderson, 1983; M. C. Mensink & Raaijmakers, 1988;Nelson, Schreiber & McEvoy, 1992;Postman & Underwood, 1973). By these views, interference may arise under certain encoding conditions, but it does not stem from an encoding failure per se.…”
Section: Interference Resistance At Encoding and Retrievalmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…3 An alternative viewpoint is that the cognitive primitive underlying performance on both WMS and many other tasks is the inhibitory-based ability to delete irrelevant information from working memory, thus reducing competition (Hasher, Zacks, & May, 1999;Lustig et al, 2001;Zacks & Hasher, 1994). Competition may stem from previous occasions in a laboratory, as the present study suggests, from a previous list of items (see Postman & Underwood, 1973) or even, as in WMS tasks and other short-term memory tasks, from previous sets within a single series (Keppel & Underwood, 1962;May et al, 1999). In any event, it is increasingly clear that WMS scores do not measure a single construct such as capacity but instead are determined by multiple factors, at least one of which is proactive interference.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Nonetheless, the reduction in WMS seen here as a result of prior experience is consistent with other recent studies suggesting that WMS tasks are vulnerable to interference. For example, variables that influence PI, including similarity between competitors and number of potential competitors, are now known to affect working memory span tasks much as they do other long-and short-term memory tasks (see Conrad & Hull, 1964;Postman & Underwood, 1973;Shah & Miyake, 1996;Young & Supa, 1941). Furthermore, manipulations intended to reduce the impact of PI have been shown to boost span scores substantially (Lustig et al, 2001;May et al, 1999).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation