2016
DOI: 10.1007/s00426-016-0818-6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Cue-target contingencies modulate voluntary orienting of spatial attention: dissociable effects for speed and accuracy

Abstract: This is the accepted version of the paper.This version of the publication may differ from the final published version. These findings, obtained with a new manipulation and by using arbitrary non-directional cueing, demonstrate that cue-target contingencies strongly modulate the way attention deploys in space. Permanent repository link

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

1
6
0
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 55 publications
1
6
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Note that this automatism cannot be based on specific stimulus-response bindings acquired during unmasked trials because the stimulus lists used in unmasked and masked trials were distinct. This interpretation is also in line with recent evidence suggesting that the endogenous cueing of nonemotional, simple cues can be based on implicit cue usage (e.g., Bonato et al, 2016; Peterson & Gibson, 2011; Risko & Stolz, 2010; see also Bartolomeo et al, 2007).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Note that this automatism cannot be based on specific stimulus-response bindings acquired during unmasked trials because the stimulus lists used in unmasked and masked trials were distinct. This interpretation is also in line with recent evidence suggesting that the endogenous cueing of nonemotional, simple cues can be based on implicit cue usage (e.g., Bonato et al, 2016; Peterson & Gibson, 2011; Risko & Stolz, 2010; see also Bartolomeo et al, 2007).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…Specifically, anticipatory attentional process might proceed more quickly, efficiently or involuntarily when attentional cues are emotional (for a review of amygdala-based preferential emotional attention from the viewpoint of automatic attention to emotional stimuli, see Pourtois, Schettino, & Vuilleumier, 2013). While the prevailing view on endogenous cueing is that slow, strategic, voluntarily controlled processes underlie attentional orienting when central symbolic cues are used (e.g., Egeth & Yantis, 1997; Müller & Rabbitt, 1989; Posner, 1980), more recent evidence suggests that more “automatic” processes can also give rise or contribute to the endogenous cueing effect (e.g., Bonato, Lisi, Pegoraro, & Pourtois, 2016; Peterson & Gibson, 2011; Risko & Stolz, 2010; see also Bartolomeo, Decaix, & Siéroff, 2007). Specifically, this view assumes that in a typical endogenous cueing paradigm, participants are able to allocate their attention on the basis of implicit learning of cue-dependent target location probabilities, that is, without or independently of conscious effort or awareness of these contingencies, at least if simple central cues are used—such as color cues or cues with intrinsic spatial meaning—because salient cue information might facilitate contingency learning (Bonato et al, 2016; Peterson & Gibson, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Therefore, Experiment 2 sought a way to cue participants completely endogenously. To do so, participants first learned to associate a particular colour (e.g., red) with a particular spatial location (e.g., left); consistently, in the main experiment, the colour of the fixation cross endogenously cued participants to pay attention to either the left or right spatial location (Bonato et al, 2018; Meyer et al, 2018). We further explored the effect of purely endogenous orienting induced by the predictability of the cue on MSI.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Ao longo do experimento, dicas inválidas foram inseridas gradualmente até que a proporção entre válidas e inválidas atingisse a quantidade proposta. A quantidade de dicas foi de 192 válidas, 48 inválidas e 48 neutras [13] [14]. A Figura 2 apresenta a estrutura de cada tentativa.…”
Section: Materiais E Métodosunclassified