2008
DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x08003543
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Darwin's mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds

Abstract: Over the last quarter century, the dominant tendency in comparative cognitive psychology has been to emphasize the similarities between human and nonhuman minds and to downplay the differences as "one of degree and not of kind" (Darwin 1871). In the present target article, we argue that Darwin was mistaken: the profound biological continuity between human and nonhuman animals masks an equally profound discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. To wit, there is a significant discontinuity in the degree to … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

16
761
3
11

Year Published

2010
2010
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

2
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 930 publications
(805 citation statements)
references
References 386 publications
(670 reference statements)
16
761
3
11
Order By: Relevance
“…In addition to supporting generalization to new animal pairs, these explicit relations can be systematically transformed to solve analogies based on higher-order relations between different pairs of polar adjectives (e.g., larger: smaller:: faster: slower). However, such highlevel reasoning is beyond the capability of most animals (indeed, it may be uniquely human; Penn, Holyoak, & Povinelli, 2008). In contrast, basic comparative judgment appears to be similar in humans and symbol-trained monkeys (Diester & Nieder, 2010;Moyer & Landauer, 1967).…”
Section: Multiple Levels Of Representation For Comparative Relationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In addition to supporting generalization to new animal pairs, these explicit relations can be systematically transformed to solve analogies based on higher-order relations between different pairs of polar adjectives (e.g., larger: smaller:: faster: slower). However, such highlevel reasoning is beyond the capability of most animals (indeed, it may be uniquely human; Penn, Holyoak, & Povinelli, 2008). In contrast, basic comparative judgment appears to be similar in humans and symbol-trained monkeys (Diester & Nieder, 2010;Moyer & Landauer, 1967).…”
Section: Multiple Levels Of Representation For Comparative Relationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As Penn et al (2008) argued based on a review of comparative studies, there is overwhelming evidence that many species of animals can make relational judgments based on perceptual information, yet no compelling evidence that any non-human primate is able to reason about relations. At the same time, it appears that the neural system supporting comparisons based on approximate magnitude in non-human primates operates in humans as well (Dehaene & Changeux, 1993).…”
Section: Re-representation and The Emergence Of Explicit Relationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Why? The answer, which calls for advances in the theoretical integration of causal models with analogical inference, may provide a major clue as to why the inductive power of human reasoning exceeds that of any other form of biological or artificial intelligence (Holland et al, 1986;Penn, Holyoak, & Povinelli, 2008). 3b.…”
Section: Future Directionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…An important issue is therefore whether metacognitive processes, such as analogies, syntax, and thoughts about thoughts, require dynamically embedded groupings (Hummel and Holyoak 2005). If so, some phylogenetic and ontogenetic differences may be due to the evolution and development of that specific capacity (Penn, Holyoak, and Povinelli 2008). The neuronal dynamics that could embody any such capability are yet to be discovered.…”
Section: Dynamic Embeddingmentioning
confidence: 99%