2010
DOI: 10.1002/dev.20448
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Defining age limits of the sensitive period for attachment learning in rat pups

Abstract: Enhanced odor preference learning and attenuated fear learning characterizes rat pups’ attachment learning Sensitive Period for learning the maternal odor. This period terminates at 10 days old (PN10) with increasing endogenous levels of the stress hormone, corticosterone. Increasing Sensitive Period pups’ corticosterone prematurely terminates the Sensitive Period, while decreasing corticosterone in older pups delays Sensitive Period termination. Here we extend these findings and define the age range corticost… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
79
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 68 publications
(82 citation statements)
references
References 137 publications
(202 reference statements)
3
79
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Rather, these findings provide an initial rationale for exploring behavioral or pharmacological interventions aimed at up-regulating regional NTRK3 expression early in the lives of individuals likely to develop anxiety and depressive disorders. Recent studies in rodents demonstrate the feasibility of performing early targeted interventions that have long-term impacts on anxiety and adaptive responses to stress (54)(55)(56). For example, neonatal injections of the neuroplasticity-related growth factor fibroblast growth factor 2 (FGF2) altered the developmental trajectory of highanxious rodents, resulting in decreased adult anxiety (55).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Rather, these findings provide an initial rationale for exploring behavioral or pharmacological interventions aimed at up-regulating regional NTRK3 expression early in the lives of individuals likely to develop anxiety and depressive disorders. Recent studies in rodents demonstrate the feasibility of performing early targeted interventions that have long-term impacts on anxiety and adaptive responses to stress (54)(55)(56). For example, neonatal injections of the neuroplasticity-related growth factor fibroblast growth factor 2 (FGF2) altered the developmental trajectory of highanxious rodents, resulting in decreased adult anxiety (55).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One reason we cannot make this claim in humans is because the youngest age tested (i.e., 6 years old)(Gee, et al, in press) is probably too late to identify a developmental switch from a quiescent amygdala state to a functional one. The normative switch probably predates the ages tested; to begin to answer this question, preschool age children or younger would need to be scanned to identify the age at which the amygdala becomes functionally active as has been done in rodents (Upton & Sullivan, 2010; Wiedenmayer & Barr, 2001). …”
Section: The Development Of Affective Behaviors (Humans)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As depicted in Table 1, our results indicate that maternal presence in PN10–15 pups reengages the attachment circuitry during learning, effectively preventing them from acquiring an odor aversion or fear. The ability of maternal presence to reengage the attachment circuitry appears to end at PN15, as PN16 pups still learn an odor aversion even in the presence of the mother (Upton & Sullivan, 2010). Based upon these data, we now define PN10–15 as a “conditional sensitive period,” in which odor preference learning and attenuated fear learning can be reestablished if the mother is present.…”
Section: Corticosterone Amygdala Activity and The Ontogeny Of Fearmentioning
confidence: 99%