2006
DOI: 10.1097/fbp.0b013e3280116cfe
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Delay discounting in college cigarette chippers

Abstract: Individuals who smoke cigarettes regularly but do not become dependent on them provide a unique opportunity for studying the factors that inhibit drug dependence. Previous research on this population, sometimes referred to as 'cigarette chippers', showed that they did not differ from regular smokers in terms of smoking topography (e.g. puff number and duration) and circulating nicotine levels, but that they did show more self-control according to answers on a questionnaire. We evaluated the generality of this … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
92
2
2

Year Published

2008
2008
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 98 publications
(102 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
6
92
2
2
Order By: Relevance
“…We replicate a substantial number of studies that have demonstrated that adult cigarette smokers discount future gains more than controls (e.g., Baker et al, 2003;Heyman and Gibb, 2006;Mitchell, 1999;Ohmura et al, 2005;Reynolds et al, 2004). We also provide the first demonstration that nicotine-dependent individuals discount past gains more than controls.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 71%
“…We replicate a substantial number of studies that have demonstrated that adult cigarette smokers discount future gains more than controls (e.g., Baker et al, 2003;Heyman and Gibb, 2006;Mitchell, 1999;Ohmura et al, 2005;Reynolds et al, 2004). We also provide the first demonstration that nicotine-dependent individuals discount past gains more than controls.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 71%
“…Similarly, heavy social drinkers (Vuchinich and Simpson 1998) and alcoholics (Petry 2001, but see Kirby and Petry 2004) discounted monetary reinforcers more than light social drinkers and nonalcoholics, respectively. Cocaine abusers (Coffey et al 2003;Heil et al 2006;Kirby and Petry 2004), methamphetamine abusers (Hoffman et al 2006;Monterosso et al 2007), and cigarette smokers (Baker et al 2003;Bickel et al 1999;Heyman and Gibb 2006;Mitchell 1999;Reynolds et al 2004b, but see Johnson et al 2007;Ohmura et al 2005) also discounted future monetary rewards to a greater extent than nonusers. To address H2 in humans, that drug ingestion elevated impulsive behavior, it would be necessary to show that elevated impulsivity during use returned to and remained at lower levels (equivalent to nonusers) in ex-users (e.g., Bickel et al 1999).…”
Section: Impulsive Choicementioning
confidence: 95%
“…Subjects who consistently choose the smaller immediate reinforcer are said to discount the value of the delayed reinforcer, and it is possible that drug abuse may occur, at least in part, because the beneficial value of drug abstinence is discounted compared to the immediate effects of a drug [21,48]. Accordingly, compared to nonusers, delayed rewards are discounted to a greater extent in users of opioids [40,41,48], alcohol [89], cocaine [17,34,40], methamphetamine [37,56], and cigarettes [4,11,36,55,58,72, but see 47]. It is likely that the increased discounting in drug abusers compared with nonabusers arises from a combination of factors, including higher baseline levels of impulsivity in drug abusers, increases in impulsivity due to acute or chronic drug effects, and common genetic and environmental factors that predispose individuals to both drug abuse and impulsive choice.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%