1998
DOI: 10.1007/s002130050783
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Deprivation state but not nicotine content of the cigarette affects responding by smokers on a progressive ratio task

Abstract: In two experiments, we used a progressive ratio schedule to explore factors associated with smoking motivation. In study 1, smokers who had abstained for more than 8 h bar-pressed for longer to obtain puffs on a cigarette than did non-deprived smokers. Neither group, however, showed nicotine-induced improvements in performance on an attention-demanding task. In the second study, two groups of minimally deprived smokers worked on the progressive ratio task for puffs of either a standard or an ultralow nicotine … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
33
1
1

Year Published

1999
1999
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 40 publications
(41 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
6
33
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In corroboration with the incentive learning effects, human tobacco self-administration has been shown to be increased by smoking deprivation (Epstein et al 1991;Griffiths et al 1996;Madden and Bickel 1999;Perkins et al 1994Perkins et al , 1996Perkins et al , 1997Rusted et al 1998;Willner et al 1995). However, because the tobacco-seeking response was not measured in extinction, the effect of deprivation could be brought about either by the increased value of the tobacco expectancy or by the higher reward value of tobacco experienced through consumption.…”
Section: Emotional Conditioning and Nicotine Addictionmentioning
confidence: 78%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In corroboration with the incentive learning effects, human tobacco self-administration has been shown to be increased by smoking deprivation (Epstein et al 1991;Griffiths et al 1996;Madden and Bickel 1999;Perkins et al 1994Perkins et al , 1996Perkins et al , 1997Rusted et al 1998;Willner et al 1995). However, because the tobacco-seeking response was not measured in extinction, the effect of deprivation could be brought about either by the increased value of the tobacco expectancy or by the higher reward value of tobacco experienced through consumption.…”
Section: Emotional Conditioning and Nicotine Addictionmentioning
confidence: 78%
“…However, there is currently no evidence that deprivation augments physiological reactivity to smoke-related cues (Maude-Griffin and Tiffany 1996;Payne et al 1996). By contrast, smoking deprivation has been shown to increase instrumental tobacco-seeking behaviour (Epstein et al 1991;Griffiths et al 1996;Madden and Bickel 1999;Perkins et al 1996Perkins et al , 1997Rusted et al 1998;Willner et al 1995). However, there is no clear evidence that deprivation modulates smoking cue effect on self-administration (Perkins et al 1994).…”
Section: Subjective Cravingmentioning
confidence: 86%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The primary task outcome is the breakpoint or highest response requirement completed in order to obtain a cigarette puff. Similar PRTs have been used as sensitive measures of cigarette reward with deprived and satiated smokers when puffs of a cigarette serve as the reinforcer ( Donny, Houtsmuller & Stitzer, 2007 ;Rusted, Mackee, Williams, & Willner, 1998 ;Shahan, Bickel, Madden, & Badger, 1999 ;Willner, Hardman, & Eaton, 1995 ).…”
Section: Nicotine and Tobacco Researchmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In humans, a common use of the task is to determine how much work people are willing to perform for an addictive drug (Depoortere et al, 1993;Rush et al, 2001;Rusted et al, 1998). It can be used with any kind of reinforcer, including food and money Paule et al, 1990).…”
Section: The Progressive Ratio Taskmentioning
confidence: 99%