2014
DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2013.12.018
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Depth perception of illusory surfaces

Abstract: The perception of an illusory surface, a subjectively perceived surface that is not given in the image, is one of the most intriguing phenomena in vision. It strongly influences the perception of some fundamental properties, namely, depth, lightness and contours. Recently, we suggested (1) that the context-sensitive mechanism of depth computation plays a key role in creating the illusion, (2) that the illusory lightness perception can be explained by an influence of depth perception on the lightness computatio… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
1
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…He concluded their combination is represented by a mixture model that allows prior models to differ between observers depending on their interpretation of sensory cues. Recently, N. Kogo, Drozdzewska, Zaenen, Alp, and Wagemans (2014) assessed the perceived depth of planar Kanizsa figures by varying the structure and polarity of the inducers and compared the perceived depth to similar Kanizsa-like figures with inducers that do not create illusory boundaries. They proposed a nonlinear dynamic weighting model to describe the combination of occlusion cues and depth from binocular disparity in Kanizsa configurations, which suggests consistent cues may work together to enhance depth perception of illusory surfaces and reduce the ambiguity of individual cues.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…He concluded their combination is represented by a mixture model that allows prior models to differ between observers depending on their interpretation of sensory cues. Recently, N. Kogo, Drozdzewska, Zaenen, Alp, and Wagemans (2014) assessed the perceived depth of planar Kanizsa figures by varying the structure and polarity of the inducers and compared the perceived depth to similar Kanizsa-like figures with inducers that do not create illusory boundaries. They proposed a nonlinear dynamic weighting model to describe the combination of occlusion cues and depth from binocular disparity in Kanizsa configurations, which suggests consistent cues may work together to enhance depth perception of illusory surfaces and reduce the ambiguity of individual cues.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When looking at this figure, we experience something suspended, floating, a phenomenology that we do not find when we turn our gaze to Figure 1a. Here the illusory figure is mainly defined by illusory contours and enhanced brightness, attributes that do not characterize the illusory figures in Figure 1b and c. A dissociation of the attributes of the illusory figures has already been observed (Parks & Marks, 1985; Kogo et al, 2014).…”
Section: Illusory Figures and Depth Illusionmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…More than 40 years after the first investigations (Coren, 1972; Kogo et al, 2014), the depth issue related to the illusory figures still arouses interest. However, it is curious that this phenomenon has been circumscribed to a unique observational contest, that is, the white surface with the four pacmen as inducers.…”
Section: Illusory Figures and Depth Illusionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The reference patterns were composed by 12 “Pac-Man” like items but half of the items were drawn in white (RGB = 1, 1, 1) and the other half in black (RGB = −1, −1, −1). As in the first experiment, test patterns contained a variable numerosity (from 9 to 15 “Pac-Man” like items), but those containing an even numerosity (10, 12, 14) were constructed with an equal number of black and white inducers on a mid-gray background (e.g., Kogo et al, 2014 ), whereas test stimuli with odd numerosity (9, 11, 13, 15) were counterbalanced, containing one free inducer in excess drawn in black in half of the patterns (four random visual patterns) and drawn in white in the other half (four random visual patterns). Note that aligned inducers forming the ICs in test stimuli had always one black and one white inducer (see Fig.…”
Section: Experiments 2: Reverse-contrast Polarity Open Inducersmentioning
confidence: 99%