0During eye movements, the continuous flow of visual information is frequently disrupted 1 1 due to abrupt changes of the retinal image, yet our perception of the visual world is 1 2 uninterrupted. In order to identify the neuronal response components necessary for the 1 3 integration of perception across eye movements, we developed a computational model 1 4to trace the changes in the visuospatial sensitivity of neurons in the extrastriate cortex of 1 5 macaque monkeys with high temporal precision. Employing the model, we examined 1 6 the perceptual implications of these changes and found that by maintaining a memory of 1 7 the visual scene, extrastriate neurons produce an uninterrupted representation of the 1 8 visual world. These results reveal how our brain exploits available information to 1 9 maintain the sense of vision in the absence of visual information. 2 0 2 2fixation, is integrated with the next snapshot after the eye moves, has puzzled 2 3 psychophysicists, physiologists, and cognitive neuroscientists for decades (Binda and 2 4 , 2018; Ibbotson and Krekelberg, 2011; Melcher and Colby, 2008; Wurtz, 2008).
Morrone
5In order to understand the neural basis of visuospatial integration across saccadic eye 2 6 2 movements (saccades), we recorded the spiking activity of 291 V4 and 332 MT neurons 2 7 while the monkey performed a visually guided saccade task ( Figure 1A, left, see STAR 2 8Methods and Figure S1). The animal maintained their gaze at a central fixation point 2 9(FP1) for 600-750 ms and upon the FP1 offset, shifted their gaze to a peripheral target 3 0 (FP2) and fixated there for another 600-900 ms. Prior to, during, and after saccades, 7-3 1 ms small visual stimuli (probes) were presented pseudo-randomly within a matrix of 9x9 3 2 locations covering the FP1, FP2, and the estimated receptive fields of the neuron before 3 3 and after the saccade (RF1 and RF2). In order to assess how extrastriate neurons 3 4 represent the visual world we needed to trace the dynamics of their sensitivity as it 3 5 changes during saccades. The neuron's sensitivity (݃) at a certain time relative to the 3 6 saccade )ݐ( is defined as the efficacy of a stimulus at a certain location ݔ( and ݕ ) 3 7presented at a specific delay (߬) before that time to evoke a response in that neuron 3 8 ( Figure 1A, right). In order to assess this sensitivity map, we employed a computational 3 9 approach. First, we decomposed the time and location into discrete bins of ~3-6 4 0 degrees of visual angle (dva) and 7 ms time bins (resolution of probes). For the duration 4 1 and precision of our experimental paradigm, a full description of a neurons' sensitivity 4 2 required evaluation of 10 7 of these spatiotemporal units (STUs). We used a 4 3 dimensionality reduction algorithm to select only those STUs that contribute to the 4 4 stimulus-response correspondence (see STAR Methods; Figure S2). This unbiased 4 5 approach excluded more than 99% of STUs, making it feasible to evaluate the 4 6 contribution of the remaining ~10 4 STUs to the re...