13The impact of development and healthy aging on spatial cognition has been traditionally 14 attributed to a difficulty in using allocentric strategies and a preference for egocentric ones. An 15 alternative possibility, suggested by our previous works, is that this preference is actually 16 conditioned by the spatial cues (e.g. geometric of landmark cues) present in the environment rather egocentric frame of reference, locations are represented relative to the subject's body, whereas in 35 an allocentric frame of reference, locations are represented relative to external environmental 36 elements 1 . The use of allocentric and egocentric strategies is highly influenced by specific situations 37 the navigator is facing 2 but also by the navigator's individual characteristics. Among the later, age 38 critically influence the way people navigate in space 3 . 39Developmental and aging data seem to suggest an inverted U-profile of allocentric processing 40 with age. In young children, spatial navigation seems to be preferentially guided by egocentric 41 representations, although some form of allocentric coding can be present early in development 4,5 . 42 For instance, by having children aged between 5 and 10 navigating a multiple-armed maze 43 surrounded by distal landmarks, showed that a majority of children used an 44 sequential egocentric strategy spontaneously 6 . The use of allocentric strategies emerged gradually 45 later on during development, from 7 to 10 years of age. At the other end of the curve, converging 46 evidence supports a decreased use of allocentric strategies (and a preference for egocentric ones) in 47 older subjects, when compared to young ones 7-12 . For instance, Rodgers et al. (2012) used a 48 paradigm allowing to dissociate allocentric and egocentric strategies in a 3-armed maze (so-called Y-49 maze) 12 . After having learned to position of a goal starting from one arm of the maze, the subject 50 was positioned in the second arm of the maze and asked to return to the goal position. The subject's 51 strategy was classified as egocentric if the subject made the same turn as during the learning phase 52 and allocentric if he or she moved to the correct goal location. Results showed that older adults were 53 more likely in this situation to use an egocentric strategy, in comparison to young adults. These age-54 related shifts in strategy use have been interpreted in relation to the slow maturation (in 55 development) and early deterioration (in aging) of the brain areas involved in spatial navigation (e.g. 56 the hippocampus or frontal areas [13][14][15][16][17] ). 57An alternative hypothesis has been proposed recently. In a study in ecological conditions, Bécu et 58 al. (2019) showed that healthy aging was associated with an increased preference for geometric 59 spatial cues 18 . Whereas young adults relied on landmark cues, older adults preferentially used 60 geometric cues to reorient in space, at the detriment of the landmarks. This preference for geometry 61 has been linked in older ad...