2003
DOI: 10.1080/00222890309602120
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Development of Force Adaptation During Childhood

Abstract: Humans learn to make reaching movements in novel dynamic environments by acquiring an internal motor model of their limb dynamics. Here, the authors investigated how 4- to 11-year-old children (N = 39) and adults (N = 7) adapted to changes in arm dynamics, and they examined whether those data support the view that the human brain acquires inverse dynamics models (IDM) during development. While external damping forces were applied, the children learned to perform goal-directed forearm flexion movements. After c… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

7
41
0

Year Published

2006
2006
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 38 publications
(48 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
7
41
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The rate of motor adaptation in children differs notably from that seen in adults for both reaching (Jansen-Osmann et al 2002;Konczak et al 2003) and walking adaptation (Musselman et al 2011;Vasudevan et al 2011). Adaptation in children tends to be slower and its time course more linear compared with adults.…”
Section: Learning Rates and Re-learningmentioning
confidence: 82%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The rate of motor adaptation in children differs notably from that seen in adults for both reaching (Jansen-Osmann et al 2002;Konczak et al 2003) and walking adaptation (Musselman et al 2011;Vasudevan et al 2011). Adaptation in children tends to be slower and its time course more linear compared with adults.…”
Section: Learning Rates and Re-learningmentioning
confidence: 82%
“…Compared with adults, children acquire adapted motor patterns more slowly over a single training session, but can achieve a similar magnitude of learning (Jansen-Osmann et al 2002;Konczak et al 2003;Musselman et al 2011;Vasudevan et al 2011;Patrick et al 2014). This difference likely reflects the immaturity of the nervous system, especially the cerebellum (Morton and Bastian 2006;Vasudevan et al 2011), which continues to develop for at least the first decade of life (Caviness et al 1996;Saksena et al 2008;Tiemeier et al 2010).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, the present results confirm earlier reports of age-related differences between school aged children's and adults' speech motor performance (Goffman & Smith, 1999;Green et al, 2000Green et al, , 2002Sharkey & Folkins, 1985;Smith & Goffman, 1998;Smith & McLean-Muse, 1986;Smith & Zelaznik, 2004;Walsh & Smith, 2002). We can view these results within the context of the two hypothetical sources of variability in children's motor performance from the limb motor control literature, neuromotor noise (Fitts, 1954;Schmidt et al, 1979;Smits-Engelsman & Van Galen, 1997;Yan et al, 2000) and short-term changes related to practice (Deutsch & Newell, 2002, 2004Engelhorn, 1988;FerrelChapus et al, 2002;Konczak et al, 2003;Lazarus et al, 1995;Manoel & Connolly, 1995;Takahashi et al, 2003;Thomas et al, 2000).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Less refined timing, greater variability, and decreased accuracy of children's movements are also characteristic of limb motor behaviors, including simultaneous clapping and stepping (Getchall & Whitall, 2003), goal-directed reaching (Konczak, Jansen-Osmann, & Kalveram, 2003;Takahashi, Nemet, Rose-Gottron, Larson, & Cooper, 2003;, bimanual circle-drawing tasks (Robertson, 2001), repetitive finger tapping (Greene & Williams, 1993;Inui & Katsura, 2002), and pointing (Bourgeois & Hay, 2003). In the developmental limb motor control literature, investigators have proposed that different sources contribute to the variability in children's performance.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%
See 1 more Smart Citation