1971
DOI: 10.1037/h0030441
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Developmental patterns for children's class and conditional reasoning abilities.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
29
2
2

Year Published

1974
1974
2002
2002

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 47 publications
(33 citation statements)
references
References 8 publications
0
29
2
2
Order By: Relevance
“…At least two objections to this view can be raised. First, although Cox and Griggs (1982) provide an extensive review of selection-task studies (Wason, 1968), suggesting that content effects are only found in that task when material is personally familiar, studies of standard conditional reasoning tasks (including the present study) show that content describing arbitrary, hypothetical situations is effective as well (e.g., Falmagne et al, 1985;Kuhn, 1977;Roberge & Paulus, 1971). Reasoning problems in the present study were designed to access schemata describing possible states of affairs; they did not permit access to factual knowledge that could guarantee correct conclusions.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 75%
“…At least two objections to this view can be raised. First, although Cox and Griggs (1982) provide an extensive review of selection-task studies (Wason, 1968), suggesting that content effects are only found in that task when material is personally familiar, studies of standard conditional reasoning tasks (including the present study) show that content describing arbitrary, hypothetical situations is effective as well (e.g., Falmagne et al, 1985;Kuhn, 1977;Roberge & Paulus, 1971). Reasoning problems in the present study were designed to access schemata describing possible states of affairs; they did not permit access to factual knowledge that could guarantee correct conclusions.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 75%
“…Generally, the research does not distinguish between the first two elements of the content dimension. It shows that prior beliefs about conclusions and/or premises tend to interfere when the beliefs are in conflict with the direction of the argument (Ennis & Paulus 1965;Gordon 1953;Henle & Michael 1956;Janis & Frick 1943;Kane 1960;Miller 1968;Morgan & Morton 1944;Roberge & Paulus 1971;Thistlethwaite 1950;Thouless 1959;Wason & Johnson-Laird 1972;Wilkins 1928). …”
Section: A Broad View Of Empirical Results Utilizing the Proposed Conmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These include, for example, the use of counterfactual stimulus materials such as anomalous sentences when examining children's conditional reasoning (e.g. Hawkins, Pea, Glick, & Scribner, 1984;Roberge & Paulus, 1971) or the use of stimulus events which violate physical reality in some way when exploring infants' concepts of physical causality (e.g. Baillargeon & Devos, 1991;Baillargeon, Spelke, & Wasserman, 1985;Bullock & Gelman, 1979;Poulin-Dubois et al, 1996).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%