2019
DOI: 10.1167/19.1.5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Did I do that? Detecting a perturbation to visual feedback in a reaching task

Abstract: The motor system executes actions in a highly stereotyped manner despite the high number of degrees of freedom available. Studies of motor adaptation leverage this fact by disrupting, or perturbing, visual feedback to measure how the motor system compensates. To elicit detectable effects, perturbations are often large compared to trial-to-trial reach endpoint variability. However, awareness of large perturbations can elicit qualitatively different compensation processes than unnoticeable ones can. The current … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
21
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 17 publications
(24 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
(76 reference statements)
3
21
0
Order By: Relevance
“…By including both clockwise and counterclockwise rotations, there is no cumulative measure of learning; rather, the analysis focuses on trial-to-trial changes in heading angle (Figure 3a) (Avraham et al, 2019; Hutter & Taylor, 2018; Körding & Wolpert, 2004; Marko et al, 2012; Jonathan S. Tsay, Avraham, et al, 2020; Wei & Körding, 2009, 2010). Even if the feedback is contingent on hand position, learning with this method is assumed to be entirely implicit since these trial-by-trial perturbations, if relatively small, fall within the window of variation that arises from motor noise (Avraham et al, 2019; Gaffin-Cahn, Hudson, & Landy, 2019). Variable perturbations can also be employed with non-contingent clamped feedback, with the instructions providing a way to ensure that the behavioral changes are automatic and implicit.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…By including both clockwise and counterclockwise rotations, there is no cumulative measure of learning; rather, the analysis focuses on trial-to-trial changes in heading angle (Figure 3a) (Avraham et al, 2019; Hutter & Taylor, 2018; Körding & Wolpert, 2004; Marko et al, 2012; Jonathan S. Tsay, Avraham, et al, 2020; Wei & Körding, 2009, 2010). Even if the feedback is contingent on hand position, learning with this method is assumed to be entirely implicit since these trial-by-trial perturbations, if relatively small, fall within the window of variation that arises from motor noise (Avraham et al, 2019; Gaffin-Cahn, Hudson, & Landy, 2019). Variable perturbations can also be employed with non-contingent clamped feedback, with the instructions providing a way to ensure that the behavioral changes are automatic and implicit.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Even if the feedback is contingent on hand position, learning with this method is assumed to be entirely implicit since these trial-by-trial perturbations, if relatively small, fall within the window of variation that arises from motor noise (Avraham et al, 2019;Gaffin-Cahn, Hudson, & Landy, 2019). Variable perturbations can also be employed with non-contingent clamped feedback, with the instructions providing a way to ensure that the behavioral changes are automatic and implicit.…”
Section: Experiments 3: Adaptation In Response To Variable Non-contingent Rotated Visual Feedbackmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, we also note that more simple target arrangements are not uncommon (Brennan and Smith 2015;Heald et al 2018b;Hirashima and Nozaki 2012;Howard and Franklin 2016;Keisler and Shadmehr 2010;Nozaki et al 2016;Sarwary et al 2015;Stockinger et al 2015;Vaswani and Shadmehr 2013). Third, we used rather large field constants in experiment 3; lower field constants would likely spur less explicit learning, similar to the way smaller or gradually introduced cursor rotations do (Gaffin-Cahn et al 2019;Kagerer et al 2006;Malfait and Ostry 2004;Werner et al 2014). Overall, it seems likely that the explicit component plays less of a role in force field paradigms that use small field constants, multiple targets, and incentives to reduce strategy use.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This connects to the familiar credit assignment problem (Wolpert and Landy, 2012), as an error can be assigned to different representations of the body and the task. The system relies on various cues, priors and heuristics to resolve the source of its errors (Berniker and Kording, 2008;Wei and Körding, 2009), although the computational details are still being explored (Gaffin-Cahn et al, 2019;McDougle et al, 2016;Parvin et al, 2018).…”
Section: A Revised Model For Neuroanatomy Of Motor Controlmentioning
confidence: 99%