1971
DOI: 10.1037/h0031000
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Direct and implied social restoration of freedom.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

4
81
2
2

Year Published

1972
1972
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
4

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 125 publications
(89 citation statements)
references
References 3 publications
4
81
2
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Finally, the effectiveness of the restoration of choice was not appreciably affected by the perception of vicarious control over the restoration, contrary to the suggestions of Grabitz-Gniech and Grabitz (1973). Thus, the data suggest that vicarious control over the restoration may not be a viable explanation for the reduction of reactance effects found by Worchel and Brehm (1971) and the lack of reduction reported by Grabitz-Gniech and Grabitz (1 973).…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 49%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Finally, the effectiveness of the restoration of choice was not appreciably affected by the perception of vicarious control over the restoration, contrary to the suggestions of Grabitz-Gniech and Grabitz (1973). Thus, the data suggest that vicarious control over the restoration may not be a viable explanation for the reduction of reactance effects found by Worchel and Brehm (1971) and the lack of reduction reported by Grabitz-Gniech and Grabitz (1 973).…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 49%
“…When the restoration occurred after a delay of three minutes, changes in attractiveness were attenuated. However, a marginally significant reactance effect remained under these conditions despite a restoration of freedom, again contradicting the predictions of reactance theory (Worchel and Brehm, 1971).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 81%
“…For example, encountering a threat to freedom in a message will evoke reactance and the motivation to reassert one's freedom to disagree. As a result, people may have negative responses to the message, such as counterarguments, blanket rejections of the position, and negative perceptions of the source's credibility or attractiveness (Worchel & Brehm, 1971;Wright, 1986). The eventual disagreement may result from the recruitment of unfavorable cognitive responses, not from the initial motivation that stimulated them.…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%
“…In one experiment (Crawford et al, 2002), reducing the negative force-emphasizing regret associated with resistance-led to substantial agreement with a persuasive attempt (although the level of threat was not manipulated). Another experiment reduced the negative force toward resistance by having another person assert the freedom, thereby restoring the participant's freedom by implication (Worchel & Brehm, 1971, Experiment 2). People were significantly persuaded by the threatening communicator when the threat was removed by implication.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Anticipated interaction, by affecting liking and the need to interact harmoniously, should increase the reasons for compliance. Methods based on reducing resistance include making people feel incompetent to exercise a freedom (Wicklund & Brehm, 1968); having people exercise a freedom prior to the threat (Snyder & Wicklund, 1976), thus reducing the perceived magnitude of the threat; increasing the anticipation of regret that would follow reactance (Crawford, McConnell, Lewis, & Sherman, 2002); and seeing another person reassert the threatened freedom, thus reducing the threat to one's own freedom by implication (Worchel & Brehm, 1971).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%