2021
DOI: 10.1007/s10071-021-01509-0
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Dissociating the effects of delay and interference on dog (Canis familiaris) working memory

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Previously, working memory has been defined as a cognitive skill enabling individuals to temporarily hold information in a state of increased accessibility (Cowan 2017 ). Cognitive tasks used in dogs to measure working memory typically include different versions of delayed response tasks (Bray et al 2020a ; Fiset et al 2003 ; Krichbaum et al 2021 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previously, working memory has been defined as a cognitive skill enabling individuals to temporarily hold information in a state of increased accessibility (Cowan 2017 ). Cognitive tasks used in dogs to measure working memory typically include different versions of delayed response tasks (Bray et al 2020a ; Fiset et al 2003 ; Krichbaum et al 2021 ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Based on Fiset et al’s (2003) finding that dogs could remember the position of a displaced stimulus for up to 240 s when not controlling for the use of nonmnemonic strategies, we thought it reasonable to predict that the dogs in the current study would remain above chance at 90 s in the regular study condition but that the forgetting function would appear steeper in the nonmnemonic control condition. In the absence of similar SWM tasks, our prediction of WM duration (assessed in the nonmnemonic control condition) was based on previous studies that implemented the matching-to-sample task to assess WM in dogs and found that performance dropped to chance levels between 60 and 90 s for auditory (Kuśmierek & Kowalska, 2002) and odor stimuli (Krichbaum et al, in press). Although it was surprising that dogs remained above chance on delays of up to 90 s in the nonmnemonic control condition, it is possible that the increased difficulty of the conditional discrimination or the lack of power due to small sample sizes in the previous matching-to-sample studies contributed to the shorter duration (eight and five dogs, respectively).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous studies that assessed the duration of dog SWM using the VDT found that dogs performed above chance for delays up to 240 s; therefore, we predicted that dogs would remain above chance for the highest delay tested (90 s) on the regular study condition (Fiset et al, 2003). In contrast, we hypothesized that dogs would not remain above chance for the highest delay tested in the nonmnemonic control condition based on previous studies with dogs that found a steep decrease in performance between 60 and 90 s on delayed matching-to-sample tasks (Krichbaum et al, in press; Kuśmierek & Kowalska, 2002).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The subjects in this study were six domestic dogs, similar to the sample size of previous studies of same-different conceptualization or MTS that have included four to five dogs, two dolphins, one parrot, and two to nine rhesus monkeys (Katz et al, 2002; Krichbaum et al, 2021; Lazarowski et al, 2021; Mercado et al, 2000; Pepperberg, 1987; Pietrzykowska & Sołtysik, 1975; Wright et al, 1990). All subjects were pet dogs of various age, breed, and sex with various training histories, and all were trained and tested in the Neural and Cognitive Plasticity laboratory on campus at the University at Buffalo.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%