1987
DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.13.3.392
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Distinguishing between random and nonrandom events.

Abstract: Subjects judged whether binary strings had been generated by a random or a noarandom process. Half of the strings were generated by a Bernoulli process with p ~ .5. The other half were generated by either a repetition-biased process or an alternation-biased process. Subjects were (a) not informed about the nonrandom process, (b) informed about the qualitative nature of the process, or (c) given accurate feedback after each trial about the generating process. The data show that subjects equate long runs and sym… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

8
107
1
1

Year Published

1989
1989
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 114 publications
(117 citation statements)
references
References 12 publications
8
107
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The recency expectations are not symmetrical; sequences with zero recency (alternation rate of .5) were attributed to human skilled performance rather than inanimate chance. Studies of perceived randomness (e.g., Falk, 1975Falk, , 1981Gilovich et al, 1985;Lopes & Oden, 1987) also show that alternation rates of .5 are not judged as random. The precise contribution of the differing expectations for each type of sequence to the judgments studied here remains to be determined; plainly though, and in keeping with our account of the gambler's fallacy and the hot hand fallacy, people have distinct prior expectations for the characteristic recency associated with different classes of processes generating sequential outputs.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The recency expectations are not symmetrical; sequences with zero recency (alternation rate of .5) were attributed to human skilled performance rather than inanimate chance. Studies of perceived randomness (e.g., Falk, 1975Falk, , 1981Gilovich et al, 1985;Lopes & Oden, 1987) also show that alternation rates of .5 are not judged as random. The precise contribution of the differing expectations for each type of sequence to the judgments studied here remains to be determined; plainly though, and in keeping with our account of the gambler's fallacy and the hot hand fallacy, people have distinct prior expectations for the characteristic recency associated with different classes of processes generating sequential outputs.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Under these circumstances, expectations with negative recency have some validity because observing a particular outcome lowers the chances of observing that outcome the next time. Accordingly, a number of authors have suggested that the experience of negative recency in life might be responsible for the gambler's fallacy in experimental tasks where subjects are asked to generate or recognize random sequences (Ayton, Hunt, & Wright, 1989, 1991Lopes, 1982;Lopes & Oden, 1987;Neuringer, 1989;Triesman & Faulkner, 1990). For example, Pinker (1997) is critical of the presumption of faulty reasoning typically accompanying observations of the gambler's fallacy:…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, traders have difficulty dealing with variability in general, which may have a deleterious effect on their abilities to properly use forecasts (Lopes & Oden, 1987).…”
Section: Forecast Acquisition Decisionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the case of random-number generation by human subjects, biases may be due to misconceptions concerning what "random" means. However, some biases can be overcome through feedback and instructions (Lopes & Oden, 1987;Neuringer, 1986).…”
Section: Summary Of Tripartite Theorymentioning
confidence: 99%