2019
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0225113
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Does prior knowledge of food fraud affect consumer behavior? Evidence from an incentivized economic experiment

Abstract: This study uses a laboratory experiment to examine whether prior knowledge of food fraud persistently affects consumer behavior. We invited regular consumers of olive oil to participate in an olive oil valuation experiment. We used a within-subject design to compare consumers’ willingness to pay (WTP) for Italian extra virgin olive oil (EVOO) before and after receiving information about labeling scandals in the Italian olive oil industry. After the first round of bidding, but before introducing information abo… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

0
15
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
0
15
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, more and more evidence has surfaced, showing that individuals tend to overstate the amount they are willing to pay for a good in a hypothetical setting compared to situations when there is financial consequentiality (e.g., [1,2]). In response to increased skepticism regarding incentive compatibility in hypothetical settings, incentivized, non-hypothetical experimental auctions and choice experiments have become popular and are frequently used by empirical economists in their field and laboratory experiments (e.g., [3][4][5][6][7]). While non-hypothetical experiments certainly mitigate hypothetical bias, they often suffer from the limitation of relatively small sample sizes or specific groups (e.g., university students, convenience samples, etc.)…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, more and more evidence has surfaced, showing that individuals tend to overstate the amount they are willing to pay for a good in a hypothetical setting compared to situations when there is financial consequentiality (e.g., [1,2]). In response to increased skepticism regarding incentive compatibility in hypothetical settings, incentivized, non-hypothetical experimental auctions and choice experiments have become popular and are frequently used by empirical economists in their field and laboratory experiments (e.g., [3][4][5][6][7]). While non-hypothetical experiments certainly mitigate hypothetical bias, they often suffer from the limitation of relatively small sample sizes or specific groups (e.g., university students, convenience samples, etc.)…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Third, no measure of previous knowledge regarding the cheese products was implemented in the experiment. Future research should focus on determining how previous knowledge interacts with consumer valuation behavior [ 93 ].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In a recent Italian study [11], even though 73% of the participants stated that they knew the characteristics of EVOO, only just above 50% were able to correctly recognize those characteristics that distinguish this product. In general, previous surveys in Greece have described consumers' preferences, attitudes, and perceptions about olive oil characteristics [19]; consumers' perception regarding olive oil authenticity [20]; and the economic and socio-spatial attributes that affect consumers' choices on purchasing olive oil from the supermarket or from a friend/relative or consuming their own production [10]. Overall, many of the attributes that guide consumer preference and purchase are conveyed to the consumers through the label information and the certification logos [11].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%