2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.02.011
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Eating less from bigger packs: Preventing the pack size effect with diet primes

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
44
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

3
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 21 publications
(45 citation statements)
references
References 65 publications
1
44
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Seeing or consuming healthy foods has been shown to reduce intake of unhealthy foods, mostly among dieters (Buckland, Finlayson, & Hetherington, 2013a, 2013b; Buckland, Finlayson, Edge, & Hetherington, 2014). Similarly, health primes such as dieting advertisements, compared to control or indulgent food advertisements, can prevent overeating on unhealthy foods, again especially among dieters, and especially in the afternoon (Anschutz, Van Strien, & Engels, 2011; Boland, Connell, & Vallen, 2013; Versluis & Papies, 2016). Other weight-related cues, such as the presence of scales or very slim sculptures, have been found to prevent participants from modelling the overeating on unhealthy foods displayed by a confederate, and to reduce unhealthy consumption more generally (Brunner, 2010; Brunner & Siegrist, 2012).…”
Section: Interventions To Affect Nonconscious Regulation Of Health Bementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Seeing or consuming healthy foods has been shown to reduce intake of unhealthy foods, mostly among dieters (Buckland, Finlayson, & Hetherington, 2013a, 2013b; Buckland, Finlayson, Edge, & Hetherington, 2014). Similarly, health primes such as dieting advertisements, compared to control or indulgent food advertisements, can prevent overeating on unhealthy foods, again especially among dieters, and especially in the afternoon (Anschutz, Van Strien, & Engels, 2011; Boland, Connell, & Vallen, 2013; Versluis & Papies, 2016). Other weight-related cues, such as the presence of scales or very slim sculptures, have been found to prevent participants from modelling the overeating on unhealthy foods displayed by a confederate, and to reduce unhealthy consumption more generally (Brunner, 2010; Brunner & Siegrist, 2012).…”
Section: Interventions To Affect Nonconscious Regulation Of Health Bementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Two conditions need to be met to satisfy this criterion: the individual needs to know which behaviours, in the given situation, would serve the health goal, and those behaviours need to be accessible. In health goal priming studies so far, participants were assumed to be aware that salads are healthier restaurant options (Papies & Veling, 2013), that dieting is best pursued by eating less M&M’s (Versluis & Papies, 2016) and that healthier shopping means buying less high-calorie snacks (Papies et al, 2014). In other settings, however, the healthy behaviour may not always be obvious, and individuals with less nutrition knowledge, often associated with a lower level of education, may not be able to identify them well (e.g., Parmenter, Waller, & Wardle, 2000; Wardle, Parmenter, & Waller, 2000).…”
Section: Applying Health Goal Primesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Goal priming has been studied in a variety of domains. In health behaviour, primes referring to healthy eating and dieting have been shown to direct eye movements towards healthy options (van der Laan, Papies, Hooge, & Smeets, 2017), trigger healthy menu choices (Papies & Veling, 2013), reduce unhealthy purchases in the grocery store (Papies, Potjes, Keesman, Schwinghammer, & van Koningsbruggen, 2014) and at vending machines (Stöckli, Stämpfli, Messner, & Brunner, 2016), and curb a variety of unhealthy snacking behaviours (e.g., Anschutz, Van Strien, & Engels, 2008;Buckland, Finlayson, Edge, & Hetherington, 2014;Papies & Hamstra, 2010;Versluis & Papies, 2016a). In environmental behaviour, goal primes reminding participants of the importance of waste reduction increased choices of unpackaged products (Tate, Stewart, & Daly, 2014).…”
Section: Implementation Intentionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In support, laboratory studies have reported that compared to control cues, exposure to weight control cues have reduced subsequent food intake in general samples [ 9 , 10 ] and in individuals with strong weight control goals such as restrained eaters [ 11 ] and dieters [ 12 , 13 ]. Other studies have reported that the effects of weight control cues are moderated by particular settings such as the time of day (effects in general sample) [ 14 ] and portion size (effects in restrained eaters only) [ 15 ]. The effects of weight control cues on food intake (in restrained eaters) have also been found in real world settings such as in response to a ‘slimming poster’ displayed on the entrance to a butcher’s store [ 16 ].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%