2006
DOI: 10.1027/1618-3169.53.1.77
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Effects of Belief and Logic on Syllogistic Reasoning

Abstract: Studies of syllogistic reasoning have demonstrated a nonlogical tendency for people to endorse more believable conclusions than unbelievable ones. This belief bias effect is more dominant on invalid syllogisms than valid ones, giving rise to a logic by belief interaction. We report an experiment in which participants' eye movements were recorded in order to provide insights into the nature and time course of the reasoning processes associated with manipulations of conclusion validity and believability. Our mai… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

1
25
2

Year Published

2009
2009
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 67 publications
(28 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
1
25
2
Order By: Relevance
“…One key parallel concerned the observation that ‘conflict’ problems in both paradigms show inflated response times relative to non-conflict problems (cf. Thompson et al, 2003; Ball et al, 2006; Stupple and Ball, 2008; Stupple et al, 2011), which is entirely in line with dual-process predictions and attests to the value of obtaining response-time data as a way to inform theorizing. Stupple et al’s (2013) study also revealed that the supposedly ‘intuitively obvious’ deduction of double negation elimination (see Rips, 1994, pp.…”
Section: The Importance Of Data Triangulation In Evaluating the Normasupporting
confidence: 66%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…One key parallel concerned the observation that ‘conflict’ problems in both paradigms show inflated response times relative to non-conflict problems (cf. Thompson et al, 2003; Ball et al, 2006; Stupple and Ball, 2008; Stupple et al, 2011), which is entirely in line with dual-process predictions and attests to the value of obtaining response-time data as a way to inform theorizing. Stupple et al’s (2013) study also revealed that the supposedly ‘intuitively obvious’ deduction of double negation elimination (see Rips, 1994, pp.…”
Section: The Importance Of Data Triangulation In Evaluating the Normasupporting
confidence: 66%
“…A further monitoring approach for examining the dynamic aspects of reasoning that we are particularly enthusiastic about is to deploy eye-tracking (e.g., Ball et al, 2003, 2006) to determine the moment-by-moment attentional shifts in processing that arise when participants attempt the visually presented problems that are typically used in reasoning studies (see Ball, 2013a, for a recent summary of key findings deriving from eye-tracking research in reasoning). Eye-tracking studies have, we contend, provided some of the most compelling evidence to date that Type 2 analytic reasoning that is attuned to normative principles plays an important role in determining whether heuristically cued cards are subsequently selected or rejected in the Wason four-card selection task (see Evans and Ball, 2010).…”
Section: The Importance Of Data Triangulation In Evaluating the Normamentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Klauer et al (2000) suggest, for example, that it may be particularly difficult to construct a coherent semantic model when the elements of a problem conflict with prior beliefs. This might appear at odds with findings that believable invalid conclusions produce the slowest judgments (Ball, Phillips, Wade, & Quayle, 2006;Stupple et al, 2011;Thompson, Striemer, Reikoff, Gunter, & Campbell, 2003). However, overall response time includes not only the construction of a problem representation but also the decision process that follows.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…But beliefs do affect one-model syllogisms too (Oakhill, Johnson-Laird, & Garnham, 1989;Gilinsky & Judd, 1994). One reason for this effect is that individuals examine the plausibility of their conclusions and are likely to be more cautious in accepting incredible conclusions than in accepting credible ones (Ball, Philips, Wade, & Quayle, 2006;Oakhill et al, 1989;Thompson, Striemer, Reikoff, Gunter, & Campbell, 2003). Their caution is a kind of response bias, but as Garnham and Oakhill (2005) showed, it is not of a kind that Klauer, Musch, and Naumer (2000) included in their multinomial model of individual differences.…”
Section: Khemlani and Johnson-lairdmentioning
confidence: 99%