2013
DOI: 10.1037/a0032398
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The SDT model of belief bias: Complexity, time, and cognitive ability mediate the effects of believability.

Abstract: When people evaluate conclusions, they are often influenced by prior beliefs. Prevalent theories claim that belief bias affects the quality of syllogistic reasoning. However, recent work by Dube, Rotello, and Heit (2010) has suggested that belief bias may be a simple response bias. In Experiment 1, receiver operating characteristic analysis revealed that believability affected accuracy for complex but not for simple syllogisms. In Experiment 2, the effect of believability on accuracy disappeared when judgments… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

14
100
3

Year Published

2014
2014
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 50 publications
(117 citation statements)
references
References 28 publications
14
100
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Their data was consistent with the idea that belief bias may just be a response bias. Trippas, Handley, and Verde (2013), using SDT, only partially replicated their finding. In contrast to the lower cognitive ability (CA) participants who operated according to the response bias account, higher CA participants showed more signs of motivated reasoning These findings were replicated and extended in an additional study using the forced-choice reasoning paradigm (Trippas, Verde, & Handley, 2014b).…”
Section: ó 2015 Taylor and Francissupporting
confidence: 71%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Their data was consistent with the idea that belief bias may just be a response bias. Trippas, Handley, and Verde (2013), using SDT, only partially replicated their finding. In contrast to the lower cognitive ability (CA) participants who operated according to the response bias account, higher CA participants showed more signs of motivated reasoning These findings were replicated and extended in an additional study using the forced-choice reasoning paradigm (Trippas, Verde, & Handley, 2014b).…”
Section: ó 2015 Taylor and Francissupporting
confidence: 71%
“…Thus, the incongruent base-rate problem is considered an ACS measure because it cues an intuitive response based on the stereotypical information that requires an extra level of analytic reasoning to override, whereas the neutral base-rate problem is considered a CA measure because it assesses one's ability to use base-rate information in judgement (see De Neys & Glumicic, 2008;Pennycook, Trippas, Handley, & Thompson, 2014b). Based on our previous work (Trippas et al, 2013), we predict systematic individual differences in the tendency to show motivated reasoning as a function of analytic factors. If the quality hypothesis holds, CA should be a better predictor of motivated reasoning than ACS.…”
Section: Individual Differences In Reasoningmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Dube, Rotello, and Heit (2010) have made an analogous claim regarding the belief bias in syllogistic reasoning. Dube et al (2010) argued that the tendency to judge a syllogism with a believable conclusion as valid is due to an effect of believability on the response criterion, rather than an effect on the perception of the argument's strength (cf., Trippas, Handley, & Verde, 2013). We acknowledge that further research is clearly needed to explore this issue in the context of Bayesian reasoning.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In fact, a recent approach using the Signal Detection theory (e.g., Dube, Rotello, and Heit, 2010) has suggested that belief bias may be explicable solely in terms of response bias, with no reasoning component. However, evidence for the lack of a reasoning component is ambiguous (see Trippas, Handley, & Verde, 2013).…”
mentioning
confidence: 98%